|
Okay, let's let's get started. |
|
|
|
Everyone. |
|
|
|
Can everyone hear me? |
|
|
|
I'm not used to speaking in such a big lecture |
|
|
|
theatre. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
I'm Dan Bender. |
|
|
|
I'm the associate professor of Behavioural Neuroscience. |
|
|
|
My lab studies sleep, among other things. |
|
|
|
And that's what I'm going to talk about today. |
|
|
|
I do have to leave at a little bit before |
|
|
|
11 for sure. |
|
|
|
So if there are any lingering questions, I'll have my |
|
|
|
email address over here and you can email me. |
|
|
|
But my P.A. soon is operating and I can't miss |
|
|
|
her presentation. |
|
|
|
So we'll we'll jump right in. |
|
|
|
So before I talk about sleep, okay, which is a |
|
|
|
very interesting topic, I want to take something. |
|
|
|
Go back a little more basic and ask the question, |
|
|
|
why do animals have a brain? |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
That's that's probably one of the most basic things in |
|
|
|
neuroscience. |
|
|
|
Why do we have a brain? |
|
|
|
And we look through that animal kingdom. |
|
|
|
We can see that you have sort of the most |
|
|
|
primitive species, such as sponges. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And these actually don't have any neurones at all. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So the first animals did not have neurones. |
|
|
|
It happened some point in evolution. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And you see that neurones appear around the time that |
|
|
|
jellyfish and similar creatures developed in evolution. |
|
|
|
And then if we get to more complex animals, it's |
|
|
|
no longer just neurones, but we have ganglia and brain. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So it's clusters of neurones working together to do something |
|
|
|
that individual neurones can't. |
|
|
|
Now, this is where it gets interesting that if we |
|
|
|
look at our closest one of our closest invertebrate invertebrate |
|
|
|
relatives, the starfish. |
|
|
|
So the starfish is closer than roundworms and molluscs and |
|
|
|
insects. |
|
|
|
It's closer to us than all those other animals. |
|
|
|
Starfish actually lost the ganglia. |
|
|
|
They lost the brain. |
|
|
|
They just have neurones that are connected together in a |
|
|
|
ring. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Same with sea urchins. |
|
|
|
Next time you have uni for sushi, you're thinking you're |
|
|
|
eating one of your closest relatives in the invertebrate kingdom. |
|
|
|
Now, where it gets very interesting is sea squirts. |
|
|
|
The sea squirts actually have a brain in the larval |
|
|
|
stage, but then they lose it when they're an adult. |
|
|
|
And if we look at sea squirts, it gives us |
|
|
|
an answer to the question, Why do we have a |
|
|
|
brain? |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So if we look at a larval sea squirt, it |
|
|
|
looks a bit like a tadpole. |
|
|
|
It has two little eyes spots. |
|
|
|
It has a tail. |
|
|
|
It's not a spine, it's not a vertebrate, but it |
|
|
|
has a new record, which is, you know, very close |
|
|
|
to what vertebrates would have. |
|
|
|
And it has a digestive system. |
|
|
|
And its main goal and its early life is to |
|
|
|
find a nice rock, because once it finds a nice |
|
|
|
rock, it's going to attach that rock for the rest |
|
|
|
of its life. |
|
|
|
And that rock is where it gets all its food |
|
|
|
and acts as a filter filtering the water. |
|
|
|
So if you place a bad rock, it kind of |
|
|
|
sucks, right? |
|
|
|
That's the rest of his life. |
|
|
|
It's there. |
|
|
|
So the thing that's so interesting is that when you're |
|
|
|
attached to a rock, why would you need to see |
|
|
|
it if a predator was coming? |
|
|
|
You can't escape from it. |
|
|
|
You're stuck to the rock. |
|
|
|
So vision is useless. |
|
|
|
Motion is useless, right? |
|
|
|
You're just stuck to the rock. |
|
|
|
So you're just floating in the water. |
|
|
|
So what does a sea squirt do? |
|
|
|
It eats its nervous system. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
It doesn't need it anymore. |
|
|
|
It's useful energy. |
|
|
|
It digests it. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So here's the thing. |
|
|
|
If you don't need to move, you don't need to |
|
|
|
see. |
|
|
|
You don't need a brain. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So why do animals have a brain? |
|
|
|
Animals need a brain to convert sensation to action. |
|
|
|
A jellyfish does that and moves around the water looking |
|
|
|
for food. |
|
|
|
A sponge doesn't. |
|
|
|
It just filters the water. |
|
|
|
Doesn't need a brain. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now, this is where it leads to sleep. |
|
|
|
Every animal thus far that we know of, if. |
|
|
|
If an animal moves, it sleeps. |
|
|
|
Okay? |
|
|
|
Sleep is a really primitive aspect of the brain. |
|
|
|
How do we measure sleep? |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
You can't measure it in a jellyfish. |
|
|
|
It seems know when you wake it up, it snores. |
|
|
|
You have no way of measuring it, you would think. |
|
|
|
But it turns out that the most primitive forms of |
|
|
|
sleep there is a lack of motion for a prolonged |
|
|
|
period of time. |
|
|
|
Much like us, there is an elevated threshold to react |
|
|
|
to sensory stimulus. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So if I mention your name now, you're. |
|
|
|
You know, you'll look around, right? |
|
|
|
You'll hear me. |
|
|
|
If you were sleeping in class, you might not unless |
|
|
|
I was loud enough. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
Don't worry. |
|
|
|
I won't test that out. |
|
|
|
The third thing, and this is a bit harder to |
|
|
|
test, but you can test in a lot of animals. |
|
|
|
There is a rebound after sleep deprivation. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
Which. |
|
|
|
Which makes sense. |
|
|
|
If I. |
|
|
|
If I keep you for three days without sleeping, you're |
|
|
|
probably going to fall asleep really, really fast and sleep |
|
|
|
for a longer period of time to compensate. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So those there is of course, sleep is a lot |
|
|
|
more complex. |
|
|
|
When you talk about mammals. |
|
|
|
But the most basic definition of sleep are these three |
|
|
|
things. |
|
|
|
So now you can ask the question when you move |
|
|
|
up the food chain in evolution, how to sleep in |
|
|
|
the animal kingdom. |
|
|
|
So let's start with the jellyfish. |
|
|
|
This is the upside-down jellyfish. |
|
|
|
I believe this was a was an undergraduate project from |
|
|
|
a bunch of students, and it got published in Current |
|
|
|
biology. |
|
|
|
That's the story I heard. |
|
|
|
So they had a bunch of jellyfish in an A |
|
|
|
tank and they tracked the activity of the jellyfish. |
|
|
|
And they found that in the daytime it was moving |
|
|
|
a lot. |
|
|
|
And the Night-Time it wasn't moving so much. |
|
|
|
Now, that does not mean that the animal is sleeping |
|
|
|
on its own. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
During this class, some of you might not be moving |
|
|
|
a lot. |
|
|
|
It doesn't mean that you're sleeping. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So how can you tell the difference? |
|
|
|
So what they did is they perturb the animal so |
|
|
|
it couldn't rest. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
There would be a gust of water coming up. |
|
|
|
And every time the animal was sort of, you know, |
|
|
|
resting, it would have to move again. |
|
|
|
And what they found is that when they perturbed one |
|
|
|
night asleep, there was a recovery where there was less |
|
|
|
activity than you would expect prior to the perturbation. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And even when the animal slept over here, we would |
|
|
|
consider sleeping. |
|
|
|
There was even less activity. |
|
|
|
So it was almost like a like you could say |
|
|
|
a deeper sleep. |
|
|
|
Now, this is maybe not the same as being able |
|
|
|
to record EEG and EMG. |
|
|
|
You know, all the signals from the brain of humans |
|
|
|
and monitoring their sleep. |
|
|
|
But this is the jellyfish, right? |
|
|
|
And we're already seeing some basic aspects of sleep here. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Remember, they don't have a brain. |
|
|
|
They just have neurones. |
|
|
|
That's key. |
|
|
|
Now, if we go up the food chain to the |
|
|
|
fruit fly, which is obviously a lot more complicated than |
|
|
|
the jellyfish and its nervous system, we see that the |
|
|
|
fruit fly rests more at Night-Time. |
|
|
|
You know, lack of movement than in the daytime, which |
|
|
|
is already good because usually sleep doesn't have to be |
|
|
|
12 hours a night and you're awake 12 hours during |
|
|
|
the day. |
|
|
|
There are lots of different ways sleep can manifest. |
|
|
|
But you do see that there is a preferred time |
|
|
|
for the animal to rest. |
|
|
|
And what they can test here is also the arousal |
|
|
|
threshold that if you, you know, lightly do an air |
|
|
|
popper, do something that would preserve the animal. |
|
|
|
You have to put more force to get the animal |
|
|
|
to wake up. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So an animal to move. |
|
|
|
So this is one of the key things for sleep. |
|
|
|
It's not simply the animals not moving, but it's paying |
|
|
|
attention. |
|
|
|
And you have to somehow break through. |
|
|
|
It's it's an intention to to get through the animal. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now, once we're in the fruit fly. |
|
|
|
There is one other thing that's quite cool that you |
|
|
|
can start doing that this would be very difficult to |
|
|
|
do with the jellyfish, but you can put electrodes in |
|
|
|
the brain. |
|
|
|
You can tell the animal in a ball so it |
|
|
|
walks around and you can actually record neural activity and |
|
|
|
combine this with the movement of the animal. |
|
|
|
So now you have a way to to not just |
|
|
|
look at sleep behaviourally, but look electrophysiological of what's going |
|
|
|
on, to see if there are characteristics of neural signals |
|
|
|
that that tell you, oh, the animal is sleeping because |
|
|
|
you see this animals awake because you see this. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now we're going to go to a larger animal now, |
|
|
|
the lizard and the lizard here. |
|
|
|
You know, it's it's a vertebrate, first of all. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
It's a lot closer to us. |
|
|
|
They definitely sleep. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And in the lizard, you start seeing it. |
|
|
|
Sleep is not just a homogeneous pattern in the brain. |
|
|
|
There's actually two stages. |
|
|
|
Now, we don't know if it's exactly like the two |
|
|
|
stages of sleep in humans, but it does seem to |
|
|
|
be one stage where you have synchronous activity in one |
|
|
|
stage where it's less synchronous. |
|
|
|
And these seem to oscillate back and forth. |
|
|
|
So it's quite different from what you see in other |
|
|
|
mammals. |
|
|
|
But you already start seeing neural signatures of two different |
|
|
|
types of sleep. |
|
|
|
And reptiles. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now when you get to birds and mammals, now you |
|
|
|
have a more defined sleep pattern. |
|
|
|
And this is typically called slow wave sleep and REM |
|
|
|
sleep, Slow wave sleep. |
|
|
|
You can also call non-REM sleep. |
|
|
|
But essentially, when you go to sleep, you fall into |
|
|
|
deep sleep. |
|
|
|
And you have a lot of this in your early |
|
|
|
sleep. |
|
|
|
And then you have a little bit of REM sleep. |
|
|
|
And then each cycle of sleep, which is about 90 |
|
|
|
minutes, you have a little bit more REM and a |
|
|
|
little bit less slow wave. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And by the end of the night, it's mostly REM |
|
|
|
sleep. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Show of hands. |
|
|
|
Do you typically dream in your slow wave sleep or |
|
|
|
REM sleep? |
|
|
|
Raise your hand if it's slow. |
|
|
|
Wave sleep. |
|
|
|
Raise your hand in his REM sleep. |
|
|
|
Good. |
|
|
|
So REM sleep is where you typically dream, but you |
|
|
|
can dream also and slowly sleep. |
|
|
|
You typically remember your dreams from REM sleep more. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So when you're in slow wave sleep and you put |
|
|
|
up electrodes so you can record neural activity, you can |
|
|
|
see that there's slow oscillations which relate to cortical cortical |
|
|
|
communication. |
|
|
|
There's spindles which are high oscillation signals that relate to |
|
|
|
the thalamus talking to cortex. |
|
|
|
And there are also sharp wave ripples, which are extremely |
|
|
|
high frequency oscillations in the hippocampus that help it talk |
|
|
|
to cortex. |
|
|
|
In REM sleep, you see completely different brain activity. |
|
|
|
You see theta activity in the hippocampus and you see |
|
|
|
tiger waves as well. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So if you just were watching the signals coming from |
|
|
|
the brain, you could tell if someone is sleeping and |
|
|
|
whether it's REM sleep or non REM sleep. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And the key thing to remember is REM sleep activity |
|
|
|
is very similar to awake activity that is not synchronised |
|
|
|
and sort of like all the neurones. |
|
|
|
Imagine if the orchestra and there all every instrument is |
|
|
|
just practising on its own. |
|
|
|
They don't seem coordinated. |
|
|
|
Slow wave sleep. |
|
|
|
The whole orchestra is going boom, boom, boom together. |
|
|
|
And that's what the slow waves are. |
|
|
|
Okay, so you have these two different types of sleep. |
|
|
|
Now we can look at the animal kingdom to see |
|
|
|
how does this type of sleep? |
|
|
|
Very right. |
|
|
|
Do all animals sleep like us or is there a |
|
|
|
lot of variability? |
|
|
|
And what you find is that at least with REM |
|
|
|
sleep, it is really, really different across the animal kingdom. |
|
|
|
So the platypus is the king of REM sleep. |
|
|
|
They have 8 hours a night. |
|
|
|
We only have two. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
The dolphin is unfortunately the animal with one of the |
|
|
|
least amount of REM sleep. |
|
|
|
They have minutes every night. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So we know REM sleep is really important. |
|
|
|
The same time. |
|
|
|
That dolphin is a lot smarter than the platypus. |
|
|
|
So it's not really clear why all this extra sleep |
|
|
|
a platypus is getting what it gains from us. |
|
|
|
Now, if you look at overall sleep times across the |
|
|
|
animal kingdom, one thing you notice is that two animals |
|
|
|
that are genetically very similar can have very different amounts |
|
|
|
of sleep. |
|
|
|
So a nice example is the owl monkey below over |
|
|
|
here compared to the human right. |
|
|
|
We're both primates. |
|
|
|
We actually both have the same amount of REM sleep |
|
|
|
per night, about 2 hours. |
|
|
|
But the owl monkey sleeps 17 hours. |
|
|
|
We sleep well. |
|
|
|
We're supposed to sleep 8 hours. |
|
|
|
It's probably a lot less. |
|
|
|
In general, you're staying up late at night, you know, |
|
|
|
with your Twitter accounts and bingeing Netflix, like. |
|
|
|
Like all of us. |
|
|
|
So it's not just genes, right? |
|
|
|
There's something else going on. |
|
|
|
And it's very likely to be ethology, which means there |
|
|
|
are specific constraints on animals, behaviour and habitat. |
|
|
|
So, for instance, if you could sleep all day, right, |
|
|
|
because you didn't have to worry about food and resources |
|
|
|
and other things. |
|
|
|
Yeah, maybe. |
|
|
|
Maybe over many, many millions of years, you would start |
|
|
|
sleeping more, right? |
|
|
|
Another thing that's quite buried in the animal kingdom is |
|
|
|
unit hemisphere of sleep. |
|
|
|
So we sleep with both hemispheres. |
|
|
|
At least most of us do. |
|
|
|
The dolphins are famous for sleeping with one hemisphere than |
|
|
|
the other hemisphere. |
|
|
|
And you can see this as they will sort of |
|
|
|
circle around when they're sleeping with one side and then |
|
|
|
they circle the other direction when they're sleeping with the |
|
|
|
other part of their brain. |
|
|
|
And they have to do this because. |
|
|
|
They need to breathe consciously, so they have to keep |
|
|
|
part of their brain on. |
|
|
|
Otherwise, they die. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So it turns out that it's not just dolphins that |
|
|
|
are cool, that there are lots of species that are |
|
|
|
capable of this, including birds, water, animals, reptiles. |
|
|
|
It's almost that we are the oddballs of the year. |
|
|
|
And if you think about it, it's it's quite smart |
|
|
|
that you always keep part of your brain on is |
|
|
|
if a predator comes or you need to be able |
|
|
|
to do something, you would react faster, right? |
|
|
|
Or your brain is a bit groggy, but the other |
|
|
|
part is awake. |
|
|
|
And I mean, I've even heard stories of lizards that |
|
|
|
will sleep, you know, with both eyes closed. |
|
|
|
But if they sense that there is a predator in |
|
|
|
the room, one eye opens and they switch to, you |
|
|
|
know, have a third sleep. |
|
|
|
So there's probably some flexibility, but a lot of mammals |
|
|
|
sleep in or birds will sleep in nests and they |
|
|
|
have sort of very safe habitats. |
|
|
|
And over time, maybe you have a unit of hemispheric |
|
|
|
sleep is not necessary anymore. |
|
|
|
And it's not as efficient as sleeping with both hemispheres. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
It takes the dolphin twice as long to sleep because |
|
|
|
it have to do one side at a time. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
It's not just non-REM sleep that is due to hemisphere. |
|
|
|
REM sleep requires both hemispheres as far as we can |
|
|
|
tell. |
|
|
|
So dolphins have a hard time during REM sleep. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
It's like they do. |
|
|
|
They have their little dream and then they have to |
|
|
|
breathe. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So that's that's tough. |
|
|
|
They're doing this at, you know, seconds or a minute |
|
|
|
at a time. |
|
|
|
They can hold their breath longer than us. |
|
|
|
Fortunately, when we get to the first seal, it's interesting |
|
|
|
because the first seal has a behaviour that's more like |
|
|
|
terrestrial mammals, but it also lives in the water. |
|
|
|
And it turns out that when it's in the water, |
|
|
|
you can't do very much REM sleep. |
|
|
|
And it also has to sleep with one hemisphere at |
|
|
|
a time. |
|
|
|
But as soon as it comes to land, then it |
|
|
|
says, okay, great. |
|
|
|
I don't need to consciously breathe, you know, underwater. |
|
|
|
I can have my REM sleep and I can sleep |
|
|
|
with both hemispheres and it switches. |
|
|
|
So it shows you two really important things. |
|
|
|
First, this is the this is much more efficient to |
|
|
|
sleep with both hemispheres and to have REM sleep. |
|
|
|
At the same time is not essential, right? |
|
|
|
It's hard to wrap your head around. |
|
|
|
How does an animal get away with not having REM |
|
|
|
sleep yet? |
|
|
|
As soon as it has an opportunity, it will do |
|
|
|
loads of it. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So it must have a function. |
|
|
|
We just don't know what it is. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So to sum up so far, all animals with a |
|
|
|
brain need to sleep. |
|
|
|
As far as we know. |
|
|
|
If you discover an animal that doesn't need to sleep, |
|
|
|
nature paper, great sleep structure and requirements vary greatly across |
|
|
|
different species with different ethology, the habitat, the behaviour, all |
|
|
|
of that seems to influence how the animals sleep. |
|
|
|
So there is flexibility in what a species can do. |
|
|
|
It's not like I'm human. |
|
|
|
I have to sleep 8 hours. |
|
|
|
But it might take a long time for me to |
|
|
|
adapt to then sleep more or sleep less. |
|
|
|
So the big question is why is sleep important? |
|
|
|
So you have to look at sleep and say this |
|
|
|
is the worst possible idea evolution could have come up |
|
|
|
with. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
What is your function as a species? |
|
|
|
It is to make more of you. |
|
|
|
To make more. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So mating. |
|
|
|
You need to get food for energy so you can |
|
|
|
do that. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So foraging, hunting. |
|
|
|
And then there are other animals that are looking for |
|
|
|
food, too. |
|
|
|
And if they hunt you, you're not going to mate. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
So you need to avoid predation and sleep is one |
|
|
|
third of your life and you can't do these essential |
|
|
|
things. |
|
|
|
So why would why would the brain evolve to take |
|
|
|
give you a disadvantage in these things? |
|
|
|
You must be getting something in return. |
|
|
|
But it's we still don't really understand what we are |
|
|
|
getting in return. |
|
|
|
And just to emphasise how important sleep is, right. |
|
|
|
You would probably say, well, you need food and water |
|
|
|
more. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
Well, the world record for no water is 18 days. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
No food. |
|
|
|
74 days. |
|
|
|
How long can someone go without sleep, you think? |
|
|
|
11 days. |
|
|
|
Hey, Randy Gardner. |
|
|
|
Hey. |
|
|
|
Now it's even possible that part of his brain was |
|
|
|
sleeping. |
|
|
|
There are some reports of local sleep, but I think |
|
|
|
he had problems after this experiment that he performed on |
|
|
|
on himself. |
|
|
|
And you probably have tested yourself on going one day |
|
|
|
without sleep, maybe two. |
|
|
|
It's quite difficult. |
|
|
|
You don't function well. |
|
|
|
But I can tell you, as someone who has young |
|
|
|
kids, I went through a lot of sleep deprivation. |
|
|
|
You are not the same person. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
I'm still recovering. |
|
|
|
And they have done this as well on on rodents. |
|
|
|
And, you know, you put the animal basically on a |
|
|
|
on a dish that's rotating. |
|
|
|
So the animals not just have to move quickly, but |
|
|
|
it it's not continuously awake and where it goes into |
|
|
|
the water. |
|
|
|
And within 11 to 30 days or so, all the |
|
|
|
rats died on this experiment. |
|
|
|
Now, one very important thing one can found is that |
|
|
|
lack of sleep leads to stress. |
|
|
|
And stress has a huge effect on your mental mental |
|
|
|
health and well-being. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So it's hard to know whether the ability to remove |
|
|
|
stress when that's gone, we are in trouble. |
|
|
|
And that's why we're having these problems. |
|
|
|
Or is it sleep itself? |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And it's also a vicious circle, as many of you |
|
|
|
probably realise that when you're stressed, you can't fall asleep. |
|
|
|
When you can't fall asleep. |
|
|
|
It will lead to more stress. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So sleep is as important as mating, hunting, foraging for |
|
|
|
food, avoiding predation. |
|
|
|
But we don't know why. |
|
|
|
So some ideas. |
|
|
|
All of you have been sick at one point. |
|
|
|
What? |
|
|
|
What do you do? |
|
|
|
You go to your bed. |
|
|
|
You rest. |
|
|
|
You sleep. |
|
|
|
You sleep often more. |
|
|
|
So there are some benefits to your immune system and |
|
|
|
wound healing. |
|
|
|
Now, why that is special. |
|
|
|
To sleep and not just resting in bed. |
|
|
|
There seems to be your immune system does seem to |
|
|
|
be synchronised to your sleep cycle and there is probably |
|
|
|
a lot more there. |
|
|
|
But why that's the case. |
|
|
|
Why do you have to be unconscious for your immune |
|
|
|
system to kick in and act differently? |
|
|
|
We don't know. |
|
|
|
Second thing is energy conservation and protection. |
|
|
|
So many animals are adapted for hunting or foraging at |
|
|
|
night or during the day, but not both. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So a good example is, you know, like the nocturnal |
|
|
|
primates that you find with these huge eyes. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
Those are great. |
|
|
|
In Night-Time, they see much better than we do. |
|
|
|
But if you turn the light on, they're like, Oh, |
|
|
|
right. |
|
|
|
They would not be able to hunt. |
|
|
|
And in fact, they would be a danger of predation. |
|
|
|
So the best thing they can do is just hide |
|
|
|
right here during the day, hunt at night. |
|
|
|
But that still doesn't answer the question. |
|
|
|
Why do they have to be asleep? |
|
|
|
Why can't they just, you know, hang out in their |
|
|
|
their cubby-hole and just wait for the right time? |
|
|
|
The next idea is homeostasis. |
|
|
|
So when you're learning, hopefully today you're learning or your |
|
|
|
neurones are building new synapses, the synapses are getting stronger. |
|
|
|
But if you do that again and again and again, |
|
|
|
there might be some negative effects of scaling up your |
|
|
|
synapses too much. |
|
|
|
So during sleep, it's thought that there is a homeostatic |
|
|
|
mechanism that rebalances things, that things don't go too high |
|
|
|
or too low. |
|
|
|
And while still maintaining the changes that occurred during learning. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Fourth is what's called the washing machine hypothesis that when |
|
|
|
you sleep, your brain is able to get rid of |
|
|
|
all the crap that's built up. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So what do I mean by that? |
|
|
|
Well, if you look at amyloid beta, right, related to |
|
|
|
dementia, humans have a higher risk. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
If your sleep efficiency goes down. |
|
|
|
So if you're not sleeping well, you have more build-up |
|
|
|
of these toxins, which then can lead to dementia. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And another example, you can look at how another thing |
|
|
|
related to Alzheimer's and there is a build-up of tell |
|
|
|
if someone is sleep deprived. |
|
|
|
So every time you're going out and partying all night, |
|
|
|
you're like, I don't need to sleep. |
|
|
|
I'm young, you're building up towel your brain, okay? |
|
|
|
You have a big capacity, you know, for how much |
|
|
|
you can have. |
|
|
|
But at some point you want to sleep and clear |
|
|
|
this stuff out so it doesn't build up over time. |
|
|
|
And it's not surprising that as you get older, sleep |
|
|
|
quality does decline. |
|
|
|
And these are associated with a lot of these sorts |
|
|
|
of problems. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now, if we look at the brain, this is with |
|
|
|
MRI, you can see that there are ways of activity |
|
|
|
happening during sleep. |
|
|
|
And when the activity goes up, okay, this happens slowly |
|
|
|
up and down when it goes Sorry, when it goes |
|
|
|
down. |
|
|
|
There is a CSF exchange which you see in blue. |
|
|
|
So activities in red, when the activity goes down, the |
|
|
|
CSF in the brain gets flushed out. |
|
|
|
And with this CSF, we think there are a lot |
|
|
|
of toxins that have built up and then it's sort |
|
|
|
of replenishes. |
|
|
|
So that's a bit like a washing machine, right? |
|
|
|
It's sort of flushing the brain out. |
|
|
|
And then you have new CSF filling up the region |
|
|
|
and this way you're getting rid of stuff. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
If you don't sleep, maybe this can still happen, but |
|
|
|
it seems that it's not as efficient. |
|
|
|
And there's something about sleep that is ideal for this |
|
|
|
to happen. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So I talked to you about four hypotheses. |
|
|
|
The fifth one I'll focus on for the rest of |
|
|
|
the lecture, because that's what my lab does. |
|
|
|
And I know more about that than the other four. |
|
|
|
So the fifth hypothesis where there is a lot of |
|
|
|
evidence is that sleep is important for memory. |
|
|
|
And this idea that your recent memories need to get |
|
|
|
backed up to long term storage. |
|
|
|
And this has to happen during sleep. |
|
|
|
So there are lots of experiments you can find out |
|
|
|
in the literature where there is some sort of task. |
|
|
|
I mean, there are really dozens and dozens of these |
|
|
|
sorts of studies. |
|
|
|
On the y axis, you see that there's an improvement |
|
|
|
in the task. |
|
|
|
And if you've taken a nap after you do the |
|
|
|
task, you'll do better than if you didn't take a |
|
|
|
nap. |
|
|
|
Okay, so sleeping is beneficial At the same time, if |
|
|
|
you sleep deprived someone, you'll see that their memory retention |
|
|
|
is is not as good. |
|
|
|
So sleeping is good and not sleeping. |
|
|
|
When you're supposed to sleep, you do worse than you |
|
|
|
should. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now, as I said before, not sleeping leads to stress. |
|
|
|
And there are you know, there are other factors you |
|
|
|
have to examine. |
|
|
|
But even when you do these things, sleep seems to |
|
|
|
be better beneficial for your memory. |
|
|
|
So I told you that sleep is divided into REM |
|
|
|
sleep and non REM sleep and mammals and birds. |
|
|
|
So what is what do you think is more important |
|
|
|
for memory? |
|
|
|
Is it the REM sleep or the non REM sleep? |
|
|
|
Ram. |
|
|
|
Raise your hand if it's Ram. |
|
|
|
Raise your hand if it's non-REM. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
A couple brave souls. |
|
|
|
Good. |
|
|
|
So here's a task. |
|
|
|
I'll answer the question in a second. |
|
|
|
Here's a task where someone has to remember the location |
|
|
|
of different different dots on the screen. |
|
|
|
And while they're doing the task, they smell this odour |
|
|
|
in the background. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
I believe it's not a very good odour, but it's |
|
|
|
for the purpose of getting their attention and making that |
|
|
|
association. |
|
|
|
And then when they go to sleep, they can present |
|
|
|
that odour while they're sleeping. |
|
|
|
And the idea is because the olfactory bulb has direct |
|
|
|
connections to the campus, that smell is going to activate |
|
|
|
neurones and remind the brain, the hippocampus over here, which |
|
|
|
is active during sleep, reminding the hippocampus that, oh, this |
|
|
|
is what you were doing. |
|
|
|
Think about this memory and perhaps bias your your your |
|
|
|
memory processing towards the task rather than all the other |
|
|
|
things you did during the day. |
|
|
|
So it turns out if you do this during REM |
|
|
|
and waking, there is no effect at all on your |
|
|
|
memory. |
|
|
|
But if you do during slow sleep, if you present |
|
|
|
the odour, the performance improves after the sleep session compared |
|
|
|
to if there is no odour at all. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So this doesn't mean that REM sleep is not important |
|
|
|
for memory. |
|
|
|
But it seems that slow wave sleep is where this |
|
|
|
might this process probably starts. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
The other thing is this works If you just have |
|
|
|
a nap. |
|
|
|
And a nap is almost entirely slow wave sleep. |
|
|
|
We're non-REM sleep. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So you can do these control experiments that even without |
|
|
|
any REM sleep, you are benefiting from having having non-REM |
|
|
|
sleep. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So what do you think is happening during this process? |
|
|
|
What? |
|
|
|
Why does this work? |
|
|
|
So. |
|
|
|
The brain is very complicated in lots of different areas. |
|
|
|
We're going to focus on the neocortex, which is, you |
|
|
|
know, the outside region, all your cortical areas for seeing |
|
|
|
and hearing and touch and the hippocampus, which is in |
|
|
|
your medial medial temporal lobe. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Which is in blue over here. |
|
|
|
So it's thought that during non-REM sleep or sorry, prior |
|
|
|
to non-REM sleep, when you're awake, cortical activity is happening, |
|
|
|
which is representing your whole world. |
|
|
|
Right? |
|
|
|
It's very much like if I stimulate part of part |
|
|
|
of my visual cortex, I'll see a flash of light, |
|
|
|
right? |
|
|
|
It's not. |
|
|
|
What is actually happening out there, is what my brain |
|
|
|
is doing that gives me perception. |
|
|
|
So all these signals in my brain that are giving |
|
|
|
me my conscious experience are being transmitted to the hippocampus. |
|
|
|
And the hippocampus is recording this. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And when you go to sleep during non-REM sleep, the |
|
|
|
hippocampus plays back all these memories. |
|
|
|
The cortex, of course, is not as active. |
|
|
|
It shouldn't be active at all from the outside world |
|
|
|
because your eyes are shut and you're unconscious, but your |
|
|
|
brain is actually still very active and your hippocampus talks |
|
|
|
to you in your cortex and said, Hey, this is |
|
|
|
what you do during the day. |
|
|
|
Let's talk about it. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And the analogy is that it's very much like your |
|
|
|
computer. |
|
|
|
You download content during the day when your computer is |
|
|
|
on, you're working on it, and then you put your |
|
|
|
computer in sleep mode and everything gets backed up to |
|
|
|
the cloud. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And you have these two modes. |
|
|
|
When the computer is active and you're doing stuff, it's |
|
|
|
going in one direction. |
|
|
|
When your computer is in sleep mode, it can still |
|
|
|
be active and can still do stuff when you don't |
|
|
|
need the computer. |
|
|
|
And it uses that opportunity to back up the data |
|
|
|
if it did it while you're working at least, well, |
|
|
|
maybe now computers are much faster. |
|
|
|
But back in my day, it would slow things down, |
|
|
|
right? |
|
|
|
So it's better to have these two modes. |
|
|
|
So what is the evidence that this is happening? |
|
|
|
All of you know H.M. |
|
|
|
I don't need to go through the story. |
|
|
|
But he had his hippocampus removed to try to stop |
|
|
|
the seizures. |
|
|
|
And when I said the campus was removed, he suffered |
|
|
|
from anterior grade and temporally graded retrograde amnesia for episodic |
|
|
|
and semantic memory, which meant he couldn't form new memories, |
|
|
|
and his old memories were partially affected. |
|
|
|
The further back in time those memories were initially encoded, |
|
|
|
the better his memory was, Which means the new experiences |
|
|
|
require the hippocampus and hippocampal damage does not erase old |
|
|
|
memories. |
|
|
|
So if a computer example we just had. |
|
|
|
If I take my computer. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
And I don't know, it just blows the fuse. |
|
|
|
Doesn't work anymore. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Everything. |
|
|
|
I backed up to the cloud I still have access |
|
|
|
to that's still there. |
|
|
|
I can't store new memories. |
|
|
|
I can't download content. |
|
|
|
I don't have a computer anymore, but I still have |
|
|
|
access to the old memories. |
|
|
|
And that's what we think the hippocampus is, is for. |
|
|
|
Taking new memories and storing it somewhere else. |
|
|
|
So the hippocampus is gone. |
|
|
|
You just can't do it again. |
|
|
|
You can't store new memories. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now, that's in humans. |
|
|
|
What happens when we look at rats, right? |
|
|
|
We can study neural signals in the brain. |
|
|
|
These are experiments I do in my lab. |
|
|
|
This video is not from my lab, but we record. |
|
|
|
We put electrodes. |
|
|
|
Many, many electrodes to record 5000 neurones at the same |
|
|
|
time. |
|
|
|
And when neurones, when you're listening to neurones, they act |
|
|
|
very much like instruments in an orchestra. |
|
|
|
They're all doing their thing. |
|
|
|
And when they produce the melody together, it's very easy |
|
|
|
to recognise they form patterns. |
|
|
|
And the question is what are those patterns there they're |
|
|
|
forming? |
|
|
|
What do those patterns mean? |
|
|
|
So. |
|
|
|
Okay, so you can hear the sound. |
|
|
|
Isn't the sounds of the spikes when you're recording. |
|
|
|
What you're seeing is a colour code over here of |
|
|
|
the different spikes. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And what you find is that when the animals and |
|
|
|
when a particular neurone is is responding, it likes a |
|
|
|
particular region of the track, the animals running. |
|
|
|
So I know I talked about the hippocampus as being |
|
|
|
important for memory, and this will come full circle to |
|
|
|
that. |
|
|
|
But when you're recording in the Rat, it seems like |
|
|
|
these neurones act like a GPS. |
|
|
|
And so the neurones like one location, another neurone will |
|
|
|
like a different location. |
|
|
|
And as the animal is running, you have neurone A |
|
|
|
and B and C and D firing. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And I'll give you a little cartoon over here. |
|
|
|
Here's animal running. |
|
|
|
And you see the first cell fire, then the the |
|
|
|
second, the third, the fourth. |
|
|
|
And if you were listening to the brain and you |
|
|
|
saw blue, green, red, orange, you say, are the animals |
|
|
|
running along this track? |
|
|
|
Hey, I know what the animal is doing. |
|
|
|
But then the animal goes to sleep and you see |
|
|
|
the same neurones fire in the same sequence. |
|
|
|
This is a phenomena called replay. |
|
|
|
It happens much faster, but it's in the same sequence. |
|
|
|
And because this sequence is very unique to both the |
|
|
|
track and animal's behaviour and can decode this and say, |
|
|
|
we know the animal is thinking about the track and |
|
|
|
its sleep. |
|
|
|
I don't know if this is a dream. |
|
|
|
You can't ask the animal what it's what's the conscious |
|
|
|
experience. |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
But we can see these patterns are very unique. |
|
|
|
They don't happen by chance very easily. |
|
|
|
And this seems to reflect what has happened. |
|
|
|
So just so you can hear how that sounds. |
|
|
|
And remember, that replay event is 5 to 20 times |
|
|
|
faster. |
|
|
|
So it sounds like all the neurones are fired at |
|
|
|
once. |
|
|
|
We'll look at the video again. |
|
|
|
You're going to see a neurone fire on the bottom, |
|
|
|
in the middle, on the left side, on the track. |
|
|
|
And when it gets to the corner all the way |
|
|
|
on the left, one is rounding. |
|
|
|
That's where the replay event will occur. |
|
|
|
And I'll I'll tell you when that happens. |
|
|
|
So. |
|
|
|
So it's running the blue cell fires. |
|
|
|
And another the science cell. |
|
|
|
Another blue cell. |
|
|
|
And here, that was a replay event. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And if you saw the cell activity that we play, |
|
|
|
there are actually cells active in front of the animal |
|
|
|
where it's planning to go. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Now. |
|
|
|
So the animal is not asleep here. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
This happens when the animal's awake as well, when it's |
|
|
|
sort of tuned out and not running. |
|
|
|
You see these replay events, but when the animal goes |
|
|
|
to sleep and you're listening to the brain, you hear, |
|
|
|
Shh, shh, shh. |
|
|
|
And these are all replay events. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Yes. |
|
|
|
Dear me while I was. |
|
|
|
Is the replay as fast. |
|
|
|
Yeah. |
|
|
|
So when it's asleep and when it's awake. |
|
|
|
There are probably differences in the replay, but we don't |
|
|
|
really understand those differences. |
|
|
|
But the speed is similar. |
|
|
|
During REM sleep. |
|
|
|
There's one study finding replay. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And it's the same speed as the actual experience. |
|
|
|
So maybe this is closer to what you have as |
|
|
|
a as a as a dream. |
|
|
|
The other thing is that during slow wave, sleep is |
|
|
|
very fragmented. |
|
|
|
So it's like part of the track boom animal replays. |
|
|
|
And then one second later, another part of the track, |
|
|
|
boom, there's replay. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So it's not this. |
|
|
|
Imagine a dream is you have this whole storyline that |
|
|
|
doesn't seem to be happening here. |
|
|
|
Maybe REM sleep does. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So you have a behavioural episode. |
|
|
|
Animals running along, you know, looking for food on the |
|
|
|
track. |
|
|
|
This is all getting encoded in your cortex. |
|
|
|
It's getting sent to the hippocampus to store. |
|
|
|
And then when the animal is not paying attention, you |
|
|
|
know, sort of zoning out or sleeping. |
|
|
|
We call these offline periods and there's a process of |
|
|
|
memory consolidation where the hippocampus backs up this information, the |
|
|
|
cortex. |
|
|
|
And one reason for this is that the hippocampus learns |
|
|
|
things quickly. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
And it has to decide what are the most important |
|
|
|
things to store. |
|
|
|
You can't store everything you experience in your brain. |
|
|
|
The hippocampus has to make that choice. |
|
|
|
So one thing we study in the lab is a |
|
|
|
phenomena of memory triage. |
|
|
|
Just. |
|
|
|
Just like a Annie, right? |
|
|
|
You can't help everyone. |
|
|
|
You have to rank. |
|
|
|
What are the most important cases to see first? |
|
|
|
So what are the most important memories? |
|
|
|
And during sleep, those get replayed. |
|
|
|
So what happens if we disrupt replay so we can |
|
|
|
do this by putting electrodes in the brain near the |
|
|
|
anterior comissioner? |
|
|
|
So the connection between the hippocampi and when we see |
|
|
|
this high oscillation, which is called a sharp wave ripple |
|
|
|
where replay events occur, we'd give a little pulse and |
|
|
|
disrupts neural activity for 100 or 200 milliseconds in the |
|
|
|
brain. |
|
|
|
And if we time it right, we prevent replay events |
|
|
|
from occurring. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So we can train the animal on a memory task |
|
|
|
involving different arms that are baited. |
|
|
|
And when we disrupt replay, we find that the rate |
|
|
|
of learning is slower in those animals. |
|
|
|
They're still able to learn, but the rate is slower. |
|
|
|
And you have to keep in mind that you're blocking |
|
|
|
replay just for an hour after the task, and then |
|
|
|
the animal can sleep in its own cage after that |
|
|
|
normally. |
|
|
|
So you're able to disrupt some, but it's not the |
|
|
|
perfect experiment where you've blocked all replay that will ever |
|
|
|
occur, you know, during the sleep session. |
|
|
|
But this is already a hint that replay is important |
|
|
|
for learning a memory. |
|
|
|
So an experiment that I did a while back rather |
|
|
|
than disruption, where you always wonder, maybe I have screwed |
|
|
|
something up in the brain by shocking. |
|
|
|
Can we do something that is positive? |
|
|
|
And so where we improve behaviour or we can bias |
|
|
|
activity towards certain memories. |
|
|
|
So I train rats on a task where they had |
|
|
|
to run to one side if they heard a high |
|
|
|
frequency sound, like high notes on the piano, and they |
|
|
|
ran to the other side or they heard lower frequency |
|
|
|
sounds okay. |
|
|
|
So they can do the task and it takes a |
|
|
|
while. |
|
|
|
Humans are a bit faster. |
|
|
|
Rats can learn this in a week to two weeks, |
|
|
|
but they learn. |
|
|
|
Hey, I hear the sound around this. |
|
|
|
I hear that sound runs the other side and they |
|
|
|
go to sleep. |
|
|
|
And what you would expect if you're recording brain activity |
|
|
|
is you would see replay of running to the right |
|
|
|
and replay of running to the left right. |
|
|
|
These are relevant memories for the animal to learn. |
|
|
|
It was getting a reward. |
|
|
|
It has to figure out the task. |
|
|
|
So you're going to see these replay events. |
|
|
|
Now, the question is, if I play sounds associated with |
|
|
|
the task, the sound telling the animal to the left, |
|
|
|
the sound telling the animal to go right. |
|
|
|
Does that affect the replay? |
|
|
|
Right. |
|
|
|
Just with the like, the old factory cue I showed |
|
|
|
you before that influenced the memory. |
|
|
|
So potentially, since the brain is still able to hear |
|
|
|
sounds, maybe not in exactly the same way you still |
|
|
|
hear sound when you're sleeping. |
|
|
|
Maybe I can sort of tease the hippocampus to replay |
|
|
|
what I wanted to replay. |
|
|
|
And that's what I found, that you can you can |
|
|
|
have a measure called the mean rate bias, which is |
|
|
|
basically you have more activity for these neurones. |
|
|
|
If the neurone likes running to the right. |
|
|
|
Are those neurones more active for sound? |
|
|
|
Are compared to sound. |
|
|
|
The sound telling it to go right. |
|
|
|
So the blue bar is higher, which means the neurones |
|
|
|
are more than neurones unlike to run right respond in |
|
|
|
animal runs. |
|
|
|
Right are more active when you play sound. |
|
|
|
Ah and the neurones that like going left are less |
|
|
|
active when you play sound are. |
|
|
|
So there seems to be a preference for what replays |
|
|
|
when you play a sound. |
|
|
|
And what's interesting is that this bias over here persists. |
|
|
|
So you might think I play a sound. |
|
|
|
I get a replay of just like that. |
|
|
|
That's not what happens. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
You play the sound and you create a state in |
|
|
|
the brain for almost 10 seconds where if a replay |
|
|
|
event occurs, it is more likely to be in the |
|
|
|
direction that is associated with that sound. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So you're not causing more replay events. |
|
|
|
You were just biasing what will happen when a replay |
|
|
|
event occurs? |
|
|
|
But as soon as you play another sound, the fact |
|
|
|
goes away. |
|
|
|
So it seems to be very, very intolerant to say |
|
|
|
lots of different things you're doing. |
|
|
|
Supplying a sound cue caused a greater reactivation of play |
|
|
|
cells associated with that sound for more than 10 seconds, |
|
|
|
which was the max tested. |
|
|
|
So the summary so far is if you train the |
|
|
|
animal to run left for. |
|
|
|
A high pitched sound and run rate for a low |
|
|
|
pitched sound. |
|
|
|
Playing the high pitched sound will cause replays that left |
|
|
|
at a low pitch Sound will cause replay to the |
|
|
|
right. |
|
|
|
If you do this in humans, humans, you don't have |
|
|
|
to train them to run right or run less for |
|
|
|
just two sounds. |
|
|
|
You can do things that are more complicated so you |
|
|
|
can have 50 objects. |
|
|
|
Each object is the subject has to remember the location |
|
|
|
the object should be. |
|
|
|
So they're touching on the screen. |
|
|
|
Oh, I see the cattle. |
|
|
|
I need to press this part of the screen. |
|
|
|
And each object is also paired with a sound. |
|
|
|
Okay, so you have 50 pictures, sound pairings that the |
|
|
|
subject has to remember and during a nap. |
|
|
|
So that's only slow wave sleep. |
|
|
|
They present half the sounds to the subjects while they're |
|
|
|
sleeping. |
|
|
|
Okay, So the sounds they present are called the cued |
|
|
|
stimuli and the unseen stimuli are they still have a |
|
|
|
sound, right? |
|
|
|
But those sounds were not played. |
|
|
|
Then they test the subjects with acute versus on cued. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Importantly, they do not present the sound in the test. |
|
|
|
It's only the visual objects and the person has to |
|
|
|
remember the location. |
|
|
|
And when they do this they see there is less |
|
|
|
errors in the cued sound. |
|
|
|
So there is learning, there is an improvement in the |
|
|
|
cued versus on cued. |
|
|
|
So presenting a sound during sleep from the right literature |
|
|
|
suggest that we can actually bias what is replaying in |
|
|
|
the brain. |
|
|
|
And then on top of that, in humans, if you |
|
|
|
have presumably more replay of one memory, you have a |
|
|
|
better memory than if you're replaying it less. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So. |
|
|
|
What's the take home message when you study? |
|
|
|
Maybe you should have some sounds in the background. |
|
|
|
I don't know how robust this is for normal sleep |
|
|
|
conditions outside the lab, but if you have test specific |
|
|
|
sounds, not music per se, but test specific sounds so |
|
|
|
like you have a word you have to remember for, |
|
|
|
you know, anatomy. |
|
|
|
And then you say, okay, every time I'm thinking about |
|
|
|
this word, I'm going to hear the sound that if |
|
|
|
you present that sound when you're sleeping, you'll probably remember |
|
|
|
the word pairing better. |
|
|
|
And so if yes, if you present those sounds while |
|
|
|
you're sleeping, you should have a better test performance than |
|
|
|
if you just sleep on it on your own. |
|
|
|
Now, even more important, if you don't sleep, your performance |
|
|
|
will definitely be worse. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
If there is one take home message, you need to |
|
|
|
sleep. |
|
|
|
And. |
|
|
|
This. |
|
|
|
This leads us to evidence that more replay leads to |
|
|
|
better memories. |
|
|
|
This phenomenon of replay, and that's something that we study |
|
|
|
in the lab now of how do you manipulate the |
|
|
|
amount of replay in a rats or mouse? |
|
|
|
And how does this change the performance of the animal? |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So thanks for your attention. |
|
|
|
I have time for a few questions, but. |