|
Good morning. |
|
|
|
So welcome back. |
|
|
|
Friday. |
|
|
|
Monday. |
|
|
|
We heard about stress. |
|
|
|
Today we're going to talk about social bonding. |
|
|
|
Before we kick off, I just want to remind everyone |
|
|
|
about while you're doing this module, do keep checking your |
|
|
|
anatomy. |
|
|
|
So this is a page in the anatomy and your |
|
|
|
guide. |
|
|
|
So here's the web guide from lecture two. |
|
|
|
There's a whole load of brain sections to memorise. |
|
|
|
And here is where one of them goes by halfway |
|
|
|
through, about halfway through this module. |
|
|
|
Now a bit more. |
|
|
|
So you keep playing with this. |
|
|
|
Can you tell some easy ones in this? |
|
|
|
Where's the cerebellum? |
|
|
|
Where's the corpus callosum? |
|
|
|
Where's the military bodies? |
|
|
|
That's much harder, right? |
|
|
|
So here's where it says Yeah. |
|
|
|
What number? |
|
|
|
Three bodies. |
|
|
|
So if you thought that was the military bodies, well |
|
|
|
done. |
|
|
|
But this is the way. |
|
|
|
Just keep testing yourself. |
|
|
|
Where's the midbrain? |
|
|
|
Where's the pons? |
|
|
|
Where is the body? |
|
|
|
Keep looking through this material. |
|
|
|
As you go through this course, I'm going to provide |
|
|
|
some new anatomy today. |
|
|
|
That isn't the place to do with the hypothalamus picking |
|
|
|
up on last week. |
|
|
|
That story on Monday's lecture. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
So let's go to our slides. |
|
|
|
So Monday's talk on stressed is quite negative most of |
|
|
|
the time. |
|
|
|
Stress is a part of our life. |
|
|
|
You going to have to sit in exam for this |
|
|
|
module. |
|
|
|
That's inherently stressful. |
|
|
|
But another key way in which our way in which |
|
|
|
we deal with stress is part of that is our |
|
|
|
friendship groups, our parents, our sisters and brothers. |
|
|
|
All potentially killing all these people around us really help |
|
|
|
as humans mitigate stress and other primates and some of |
|
|
|
the species. |
|
|
|
Here's an example of two types of what we might |
|
|
|
call social bonding. |
|
|
|
A parent with a child or two people who have |
|
|
|
couple together romantically in the textbooks. |
|
|
|
And the research on this topic of how do these |
|
|
|
bonds form between these two groups. |
|
|
|
This is known as affiliation, and we'll dive into that |
|
|
|
in this lecture. |
|
|
|
So there are many ways in which bonds form between |
|
|
|
people of different species. |
|
|
|
On the left at the top, down the top left |
|
|
|
is humans holding hands. |
|
|
|
In many cultures around the world, that's a way of |
|
|
|
single signifying bonding. |
|
|
|
Swans have an amazing kind of approach with their heads. |
|
|
|
And I think the cutest one of all really is |
|
|
|
the two old world monkeys who twine their tails together. |
|
|
|
So these are the monkeys bond for life as partners. |
|
|
|
And when they do that, they sit together with their |
|
|
|
tails bound up. |
|
|
|
You can go and see them in London Zoo. |
|
|
|
They have, or at least the last time where the |
|
|
|
London Zoo in the tropical house, they have two world |
|
|
|
monkeys. |
|
|
|
So these are these are some of the things we |
|
|
|
see, but we don't there are groups studying humans or |
|
|
|
a group studying monkeys, small number of groups. |
|
|
|
But a lot of the time research has been done |
|
|
|
on these animals is not rats, but actually voles. |
|
|
|
Those turn out to be a fascinating story in terms |
|
|
|
of our understanding of social bonding. |
|
|
|
And for humans, what social bonding bonding is linked to |
|
|
|
is love, the love we have for our parents, our |
|
|
|
family, our friends, our partner, etc., and our children. |
|
|
|
We don't know if these animals experience love, but they |
|
|
|
have very similar biochemical effects in their brains to us. |
|
|
|
And because we can study those in more detail, we |
|
|
|
can understand that. |
|
|
|
So I talk about social bonding in this lecture, but |
|
|
|
the technical term is affiliation that encompasses the bonds between |
|
|
|
individuals, partners who come together to produce children and the |
|
|
|
parent parents that bond. |
|
|
|
And look at the general process of that bonding in |
|
|
|
the beginning part of this lecture and turn to the |
|
|
|
parental bonds at the end. |
|
|
|
So this I mentioned this this coupling up the social |
|
|
|
bonding promotes adoption to the world. |
|
|
|
You can cope better when you've got friends, to put |
|
|
|
it another way, and stress, the social isolation which doesn't |
|
|
|
occur is linked to a whole range of psychiatric disorders. |
|
|
|
So it's an important endeavour in our society to improve |
|
|
|
social bonding, really. |
|
|
|
So here's the more of a longer and a bit. |
|
|
|
One of the key things you need to learn in |
|
|
|
this lecture is the word called specific to the ability |
|
|
|
to recognise a specific. |
|
|
|
So this is another species of the same of another |
|
|
|
individual of your same species. |
|
|
|
So as humans walking around, you can detect humans. |
|
|
|
So you as the human, the most dangerous thing out |
|
|
|
there for humans and most of their animals. |
|
|
|
So you really do need to detect humans, but also |
|
|
|
the most useful animal you probably interact with animals, I |
|
|
|
imagine the vast majority of you. |
|
|
|
So you need to be able to do that. |
|
|
|
You don't really need to be able to recognise as |
|
|
|
a human, but which humanism? |
|
|
|
Who is it I'm recognising and then do I need |
|
|
|
to remember to maintain a relationship with this person? |
|
|
|
So this all comes under this bonding process of not |
|
|
|
only do tend to find that is that is the |
|
|
|
human. |
|
|
|
I do know that human and I do need to |
|
|
|
invest time with that humans. |
|
|
|
So the there are a number of tasks that have |
|
|
|
been used. |
|
|
|
That's all fine in theory, but how do we test |
|
|
|
that? |
|
|
|
One is to look at approach. |
|
|
|
So here are two dogs. |
|
|
|
Dogs love to approach each other. |
|
|
|
Are people that dog owners know this. |
|
|
|
They love to get in and sniff each other's bottoms. |
|
|
|
They do more than that. |
|
|
|
They really get in there and get in there with |
|
|
|
them with the urine and material to understand. |
|
|
|
They do more than we do as humans. |
|
|
|
They also need to learn to recognise and said who. |
|
|
|
And there's a lot of work which cheap, cheap but |
|
|
|
very, very similar to us humans. |
|
|
|
But they have to identify the individual sheep they're interacting |
|
|
|
with. |
|
|
|
And again, that investment with sheep, how does the sheep |
|
|
|
know its mother and how does the mother know which |
|
|
|
individual lamb is its lamb to take care of? |
|
|
|
And there's been research on that. |
|
|
|
Now, how a lot of those animals do that is |
|
|
|
through smell of action. |
|
|
|
So in red highlighted that humans are a bit unusual |
|
|
|
in other primates, that our visual system is so good |
|
|
|
we can see people far off and we can hear |
|
|
|
thing. |
|
|
|
We can use that visual information to guide our social |
|
|
|
interactions. |
|
|
|
Not all other animals share that ability, and what they |
|
|
|
do is use of actions that ferments. |
|
|
|
You may be aware from popular culture are floating in |
|
|
|
the air. |
|
|
|
These are these are gas tight released chemicals that have |
|
|
|
some role in humans, but most are important for other |
|
|
|
species in terms of their their interrelationships. |
|
|
|
But beyond you know, beyond that, we have a particular |
|
|
|
organ in the brains of most vertebrates. |
|
|
|
So we have here reptiles and mammals that's shown by |
|
|
|
rats and a snake, and they have what's known as |
|
|
|
a bomb or a nasal organ. |
|
|
|
It detects in the air these, these pheromones I can |
|
|
|
relay them to. |
|
|
|
A particular bit of the brain illustrates that in both |
|
|
|
species called the accessory olfactory bulbs. |
|
|
|
So if you're smelling like a beautiful perfume, you'll likely |
|
|
|
be using your main olfactory system to detect that process |
|
|
|
that, Oh, this is lovely and it passes through your |
|
|
|
factory system. |
|
|
|
Separate to that main pathway is the access, the real |
|
|
|
factory bulbs, and they're helpful for detecting those social cues |
|
|
|
in these. |
|
|
|
So they pass that information that's shown here to the |
|
|
|
thalamus, that campus and the amygdala to various other bits |
|
|
|
of the brain that we'll come see. |
|
|
|
So this will really keep it to the brain to |
|
|
|
do that. |
|
|
|
Social detection, Who is this? |
|
|
|
And that's what we're looking for, the vulnerable nasal organism, |
|
|
|
very much in action in that picture. |
|
|
|
The two dogs, humans don't seem to have a formal |
|
|
|
gaze. |
|
|
|
Morgana starts to develop in the womb when you're a |
|
|
|
tiny baby and then gets regressed, it becomes removed. |
|
|
|
You don't end up coming out of the womb with |
|
|
|
the vulnerable base puts a set of eyes focussed on |
|
|
|
it. |
|
|
|
So the how do we test that? |
|
|
|
Once we've got that detection, how do we how do |
|
|
|
we explore that So it rodents and that's voles and |
|
|
|
rats and mice. |
|
|
|
And one approach is to take the top the two |
|
|
|
to the top one is to take that rat, expose |
|
|
|
it to another rat to play with. |
|
|
|
The two rats will play with each other a bit. |
|
|
|
I spent time in a board and run away into |
|
|
|
it during sniffing and grooming. |
|
|
|
If you put them back together a little bit later, |
|
|
|
don't just ignore each other again. |
|
|
|
They remember, Oh, it's this right? |
|
|
|
But if they're given a new rat again, explore and |
|
|
|
extending, you know, sniff each other and greet each other, |
|
|
|
and all those things that rats do when they interact. |
|
|
|
So you can measure that systematically how much times these |
|
|
|
two animals interact with each other and use that as |
|
|
|
a measure of social recognition. |
|
|
|
So if you were to damage the ability to do |
|
|
|
social recognition, a rat can't do that. |
|
|
|
We just treat all rats as if they knew they |
|
|
|
won't trust, will not know who it's mated with, who |
|
|
|
is it's daughters or sons or in terms of rat |
|
|
|
pups. |
|
|
|
It would just it wouldn't have that ability. |
|
|
|
So that's one way to measure it more. |
|
|
|
Find ways to use a social discrimination procedure that's been |
|
|
|
used extensively in bowls. |
|
|
|
So put the test participant in the neutral middle chamber |
|
|
|
with door flaps that can run through doorways into other |
|
|
|
chambers. |
|
|
|
What they'll do is have one. |
|
|
|
Another animal on one of these is tethered, is a |
|
|
|
little tether around its legs. |
|
|
|
They can't escape that chamber. |
|
|
|
Just briefly, for the purposes of a short experiment and |
|
|
|
another chamber with another one with a little tether. |
|
|
|
That's what shown here holding. |
|
|
|
It's like they can't get into the middle chamber, an |
|
|
|
experiment test whether the devil in the middle will spend |
|
|
|
more time with the partner than it's mated with recently |
|
|
|
might be. |
|
|
|
Or it might be. |
|
|
|
It could test lots of things, but typically it might |
|
|
|
be a partner that the vole has mated with or |
|
|
|
obviously spent a lot of time with to invest the |
|
|
|
time with or completely unknown vole that it doesn't know, |
|
|
|
or it could just hang out in the middle. |
|
|
|
But what this this fascinating work, there's a review we |
|
|
|
recommend you read in the reading material and so in |
|
|
|
young which really explored all of this work was there |
|
|
|
are two two different distinct species that probably more but |
|
|
|
two distinct species of vole, a montane voles, a prairie |
|
|
|
voles. |
|
|
|
There they look almost identical, but they have different genetic |
|
|
|
backgrounds and different environments. |
|
|
|
Now what you can see is this time spent in |
|
|
|
each of the chambers and we've colour coded these. |
|
|
|
So the green period is the partner one, the neutral |
|
|
|
one is the sort of purple one and the beige |
|
|
|
one is the stranger. |
|
|
|
So what you can see for Montane Voles is they |
|
|
|
don't. |
|
|
|
Let this go in the middle is just mated with |
|
|
|
this other one, but it would rather spend time on |
|
|
|
its own. |
|
|
|
It's just going to spend time sitting here grooming, maybe |
|
|
|
eating and doing what it wants to do, but it |
|
|
|
doesn't really distinguish between the two other goals. |
|
|
|
That's not true of prairie voles. |
|
|
|
They're less likely than anywhere to be in the neutral |
|
|
|
chamber, and they're very late to spend time with a |
|
|
|
stranger. |
|
|
|
What they spend most that time is with the partner |
|
|
|
they've made up with each other. |
|
|
|
That's helpful. |
|
|
|
So this shows these two different species that look very |
|
|
|
similar, have very different social behaviours. |
|
|
|
And as you can see in the wild, these, these |
|
|
|
variables will mate and the bond and they'll spend time |
|
|
|
together and they'll snuggle up. |
|
|
|
If I go right back to this early picture. |
|
|
|
That's two prairie voles together with their offspring. |
|
|
|
And they really co-invest and raise those offspring together. |
|
|
|
So that's one way. |
|
|
|
So what's different with these these voles? |
|
|
|
What's different between montane and prairie voles? |
|
|
|
And this review goes into this in a lot of |
|
|
|
detail. |
|
|
|
And so for this lecture and we've got 40 minutes, |
|
|
|
I'm just going to highlight a key takeaway message, and |
|
|
|
that is when you look at the brains of Montane |
|
|
|
voles and prairie voles is a particular molecule or two |
|
|
|
particular molecules that appear to be different. |
|
|
|
Oxytocin, the vasopressin, come on to explain those in the |
|
|
|
next slide. |
|
|
|
So these two molecules are much more abundant. |
|
|
|
They have many more receptors in key areas of the |
|
|
|
brain for motivation in prairie voles than they do in |
|
|
|
montane voles. |
|
|
|
So what's going on? |
|
|
|
So they discovered this. |
|
|
|
What's going on? |
|
|
|
Why these molecules? |
|
|
|
Well, the next experimental thing is for scientists to do |
|
|
|
is let's manipulate that. |
|
|
|
What if we were to inject these voles with oxytocin? |
|
|
|
What happens if we boost it artificially? |
|
|
|
Or what if we knock it out by giving a |
|
|
|
drug that stops them working? |
|
|
|
A lot of that painstaking work over decades of research, |
|
|
|
lots of graphs, statistics, publications, somebody just put it together |
|
|
|
in a cartoon slide to explain a lot of billions |
|
|
|
of US dollars being spent. |
|
|
|
What you're looking at the top is two voles. |
|
|
|
And what brought to this picture to me, but they're |
|
|
|
mainly work is work is on voles. |
|
|
|
And what they'll do is what this thing at the |
|
|
|
top is. |
|
|
|
Normally this slide would be even clearer, is that typically |
|
|
|
the two would be the thing with love with each |
|
|
|
other for the diagram. |
|
|
|
But the top one, the male profile has been injected |
|
|
|
with very suppresses and he hasn't before mating. |
|
|
|
So the two the two goals here typically ignore each |
|
|
|
other. |
|
|
|
In this first instance, they don't know each other that |
|
|
|
strangers. |
|
|
|
If we go back to that previous slide, this is |
|
|
|
this state here. |
|
|
|
The stranger doesn't care, the verbal. |
|
|
|
But what they've done in this experiment is inject visa |
|
|
|
pressing into the male. |
|
|
|
Now he keeps trying to snuggle up to the female, |
|
|
|
but what does she do? |
|
|
|
She runs off. |
|
|
|
She doesn't know. |
|
|
|
She's not interested in it. |
|
|
|
And so that's what this diagram, this this illustration is |
|
|
|
sort of looking away. |
|
|
|
What is this male sorry to do here? |
|
|
|
What if they inject oxytocin into the female? |
|
|
|
You get the same effect the other way round. |
|
|
|
Now the female is trying to couple up with the |
|
|
|
male, but the male is confused and running off. |
|
|
|
Normally, if they mated together, they would both show higher |
|
|
|
vasopressin oxytocin levels, but they're not. |
|
|
|
So this showing that you can boost this bonding process |
|
|
|
just by injecting one chemical that turns out to be |
|
|
|
very depressing for male voles and oxytocin for female voles. |
|
|
|
They're different. |
|
|
|
They work slightly different in these two species and humans. |
|
|
|
It's a more mixed picture. |
|
|
|
It isn't that simple for other animals, but for voles, |
|
|
|
it turns out to be these two, this to just |
|
|
|
this way. |
|
|
|
Right? |
|
|
|
So the problem here is that's the period before. |
|
|
|
This is about confusing the animals, artificially boosting bonding when |
|
|
|
it hasn't happened down at the bottom is saying they |
|
|
|
have mated. |
|
|
|
Now, these two, those should have enough eyes for each |
|
|
|
other and be snuggled up. |
|
|
|
And that's indeed what the female is doing after mating |
|
|
|
here. |
|
|
|
But they've injected an antagonist, a blocker agent pervasive present |
|
|
|
in the male and that he's not wanting to couple |
|
|
|
up and bond. |
|
|
|
So they've turned off his natural inclination to snuggle up |
|
|
|
with the female and invest time with her just by |
|
|
|
injecting one chemical change this entire behaviour and then they |
|
|
|
find they can do the exact same thing with the |
|
|
|
female just using oxytocin antagonist. |
|
|
|
They've blocked oxytocin after meeting in the female and now |
|
|
|
she's sort of in this illustration telling him he's a |
|
|
|
loser and he should get lost in this illustration to |
|
|
|
highlight what the scientists are inferring because is much more |
|
|
|
biological than then. |
|
|
|
To this, what's going on? |
|
|
|
Let's turn to the anatomy now. |
|
|
|
Let's turn to the human brain. |
|
|
|
The brain is sitting at this lecture theatre in front |
|
|
|
of me. |
|
|
|
And so here's a here's a sagittal section through the |
|
|
|
human brain. |
|
|
|
You can see the pons and the midbrain and so |
|
|
|
on. |
|
|
|
And if we zoom in on this bit underneath the |
|
|
|
corpus callosum, we have the thalamus in the middle here. |
|
|
|
Here we have a region of the brain underneath the |
|
|
|
thalamus called the hypothalamus, and we learned about it in |
|
|
|
lecture last Monday going on stress. |
|
|
|
We talked about the anterior pituitary previously as the output |
|
|
|
of the stress hormones, a hormone that goes in activates |
|
|
|
the adrenal cortical to growth hormone from the anterior. |
|
|
|
Now we're going to look at the posterior pituitary. |
|
|
|
The anterior is involved in stress. |
|
|
|
The posterior is involved in that building process. |
|
|
|
So what the scientists discovered, looking at the voles and |
|
|
|
then exploring in humans, is that there's a particular nucleus, |
|
|
|
the power of ventricular nucleus. |
|
|
|
It's very similar in your brain as a mammal to |
|
|
|
evolved, very similar. |
|
|
|
It just sits next to a bit ventricles. |
|
|
|
That's the reason that gets his name. |
|
|
|
There's also a super optic nucleus just above the optic |
|
|
|
plasma. |
|
|
|
That's where it gets its name from. |
|
|
|
So these two nuclei have nothing to do with ventricles |
|
|
|
or optic fibre optic pathways. |
|
|
|
It's just the name comes from where they're located in |
|
|
|
the brain, but they're tiny nuclei. |
|
|
|
They have more than five cells. |
|
|
|
This illustrations obviously schematic, but these are these are the |
|
|
|
bodies with cells that act with dendrites and they have |
|
|
|
axons, a good tone and an interface onto the blood |
|
|
|
cell, the blood, the circulation system, the capillaries. |
|
|
|
And they release, as is shown here, oxytocin, invasive breast |
|
|
|
and into the bloodstream from the Stewart the posterior pituitary |
|
|
|
gland. |
|
|
|
So typically this is what's known as neuroendocrine communication. |
|
|
|
So here we have our classic sounds of them and |
|
|
|
talked about in lecture three about sign ups, this and |
|
|
|
the transmitter systems. |
|
|
|
You also have endocrine responses in your body where you |
|
|
|
have hormones that operate here, like puberty is driven by |
|
|
|
our hormones. |
|
|
|
Here. |
|
|
|
What we're talking about is this other third pathway where |
|
|
|
you've got neurones that are connecting into the blood and |
|
|
|
releasing. |
|
|
|
This is known as neuroendocrine function. |
|
|
|
And that's how this this process is occurring. |
|
|
|
Now, this slide takes us on the journey from voles |
|
|
|
in a lab and injecting them to humans rather than |
|
|
|
injecting humans. |
|
|
|
They will take a nasal infusion of oxytocin into people's |
|
|
|
nose and get it through circulating up into the brain. |
|
|
|
And then what they found is that there's a whole |
|
|
|
range of different experiments. |
|
|
|
But one is that to show that oxytocin changes the |
|
|
|
way in which we treat faces, it's been a bit |
|
|
|
controversial. |
|
|
|
There's, you know, trying to get these expressed. |
|
|
|
Replicate can vary because not everyone reacts. |
|
|
|
Some people are really good at facial detection and others |
|
|
|
not so good. |
|
|
|
And these individual differences can be a challenge. |
|
|
|
But overall, the evidence weights the oxytocin and upregulate your |
|
|
|
ability for social processing. |
|
|
|
So detecting people, treating a neutral face is a slightly |
|
|
|
more positive game. |
|
|
|
Experiments where you have to trust other people to solve |
|
|
|
problems apparently are sort of elevated by oxytocin. |
|
|
|
But here's an experiment shown here by DOMS without biological |
|
|
|
psychiatry where they injected a placebo, so nothing going into |
|
|
|
the body or oxytocin or not injected these infusion again. |
|
|
|
And you can see, going back to Solomon's lectures, the |
|
|
|
amygdala responses to angry faces in the placebo condition is |
|
|
|
really high. |
|
|
|
There's an emotional response basically to these against the neutral |
|
|
|
faces. |
|
|
|
But what you see is that this is dampened so |
|
|
|
you get less reactivity, too fearful or angry, but in |
|
|
|
fact happy faces. |
|
|
|
So the whole thing sort of reactivity in the amygdala |
|
|
|
is lowered. |
|
|
|
That means that you're more likely to approach people, you're |
|
|
|
more likely to engage in activities of behaviours. |
|
|
|
That bonding process is what's argued by these authors. |
|
|
|
And there's a review in 2009 in Frontiers in neuro |
|
|
|
and chronology. |
|
|
|
So just to take that from a lab experiment, injecting |
|
|
|
people in the lab looking at brain scans is a |
|
|
|
real world example like I gave in the Fitbit on |
|
|
|
Monday. |
|
|
|
This is a couple of Nick Fleming and Linda Gaddis, |
|
|
|
who had their blood molecules, had their blood circulation taken. |
|
|
|
Here's Linda. |
|
|
|
Having her blood extracted on her wedding day, consented to |
|
|
|
a centrifuge and examined out her wedding alongside her husband, |
|
|
|
their parents, for their close friends and various people. |
|
|
|
And this is a wedding I actually attended. |
|
|
|
So is that what you're going to write it up |
|
|
|
for? |
|
|
|
The Daily Mail? |
|
|
|
So this became known as the cuddle, the cuddle chemical |
|
|
|
oxytocin in the study. |
|
|
|
And what is this? |
|
|
|
This just highlights the kind of thing that would occur. |
|
|
|
And this is to give a real life example. |
|
|
|
A month later, after the wedding, Linda got an email |
|
|
|
from Zach, the research scientist. |
|
|
|
And to her delight, the predictions were correct over oxytocin. |
|
|
|
Her husband Nick, and her experienced rise in the the |
|
|
|
love hormone with the cuddle chemical or oxytocin during the |
|
|
|
ceremony and the mother of the bride, the father and |
|
|
|
the relatives, they all boosted. |
|
|
|
But the friends were more mixed to experience the rise. |
|
|
|
But five didn't. |
|
|
|
Perhaps they were not feeling the love. |
|
|
|
So that's the way the Daily Mail has covered this |
|
|
|
kind of research. |
|
|
|
And you should be sceptical. |
|
|
|
Be careful about overinterpreting. |
|
|
|
This is the love molecule. |
|
|
|
It's a molecule that is raised and you can see |
|
|
|
that that actually does fit the predictions that these these |
|
|
|
molecules will rise in a small sample in a very |
|
|
|
particular scenario. |
|
|
|
But it's a very well studied phenomenon. |
|
|
|
The rise in oxytocin relates to social bonding, but it |
|
|
|
does more than that. |
|
|
|
So there's a really nice article called Oxytocin, the Great |
|
|
|
Facilitator of Life. |
|
|
|
And we're looking at this period here, social recognition, and |
|
|
|
we've been talking about social bonding and mate choice and |
|
|
|
feeling of trust and recognising social partners and children involved |
|
|
|
in play, but it's also involved in a whole lot |
|
|
|
of things that enable, as you can see here, through |
|
|
|
sexual behaviour, dampening aggression, potentially unintended childbirth, raising children, tiny |
|
|
|
babies through lactation and improper parenting is the mother right |
|
|
|
through. |
|
|
|
So they've argued this this whole cycle of life or |
|
|
|
maintenance as a species is somewhat dependent on oxytocin functioning. |
|
|
|
But it's not the only one like oxytocin works with |
|
|
|
others. |
|
|
|
There's things like serotonin and adrenaline. |
|
|
|
To all these molecules you need. |
|
|
|
Oxytocin appears to play this great, facilitative process that improves |
|
|
|
and enhances all these things. |
|
|
|
So that's the first part of the lecture I'm now |
|
|
|
going to focus. |
|
|
|
I've been talking about the bonding process here. |
|
|
|
I'm not going to look at this bit parenting, what |
|
|
|
happens in maternal and paternal behaviours. |
|
|
|
There's a really great review. |
|
|
|
It's quite sure maybe four pages by Dulac and colleagues |
|
|
|
in science from 2014 on the Moodle page is also |
|
|
|
a short 15 minute video. |
|
|
|
The Catherine du Lac is one of the best researchers |
|
|
|
in the world on this. |
|
|
|
Just talks through the camera about parenting process and what |
|
|
|
she's uncovered in her work and various prestigious institutes and |
|
|
|
what they've been able to do. |
|
|
|
It is absolutely remarkable when we dive into this work. |
|
|
|
So what they've shown in this work is that parenting |
|
|
|
occurs in a surprisingly large variety of vertebrates and invertebrates |
|
|
|
insects, arachnids, molluscs, fish recipients, reptiles, birds and of course, |
|
|
|
mammals. |
|
|
|
We've been talking about I've been talking about mammals all |
|
|
|
the way through here. |
|
|
|
But you have beetles that carry around their young. |
|
|
|
You have frogs that occur after they're young, you have |
|
|
|
birds and of course birds. |
|
|
|
And the most classic thing is birds. |
|
|
|
They have to look after their nests with their eggs |
|
|
|
in it. |
|
|
|
They have to pay it. |
|
|
|
Young, bring them food when they hatch. |
|
|
|
And so so there's a lot of analysis of parenting. |
|
|
|
The most common form of parenting is its female unit, |
|
|
|
parental. |
|
|
|
That means that the female is giving birth to the |
|
|
|
young and takes the majority of the role in raising |
|
|
|
that young. |
|
|
|
The male may lost. |
|
|
|
She's responsible and that accounts for that. |
|
|
|
And that's, as noted here, to carry the offspring. |
|
|
|
She has them. |
|
|
|
But there are many species that show this by parental |
|
|
|
care. |
|
|
|
Birds are amazing. |
|
|
|
When 90% of birds are shown in examples here, both |
|
|
|
parents are involved in raising the young. |
|
|
|
And that's partly to do with the one of them |
|
|
|
sitting on the eggs and looking after them. |
|
|
|
But the challenge of bringing food in for their offspring |
|
|
|
means the birds are particularly, you know, we'll spend a |
|
|
|
lot of co-investment. |
|
|
|
But that's also true of snakes and reptiles in some |
|
|
|
cases. |
|
|
|
Then they share this. |
|
|
|
But there are also some species of bird important to |
|
|
|
be aware of where there's a male unique parents. |
|
|
|
Also stickleback fish or a good example. |
|
|
|
And seahorses are not shown here, but they show particular |
|
|
|
male that the female will lay the eggs, but the |
|
|
|
male will then take over the role of raising those |
|
|
|
young to the point at which they're they grow up |
|
|
|
and become independent. |
|
|
|
So it's quite fascinating. |
|
|
|
So what's what's the takes on this new lots of |
|
|
|
different species here. |
|
|
|
But one of the conclusions of studying them in Europe |
|
|
|
biologically is that there appear to be across beetles to |
|
|
|
the to the stickleback fish through the reptiles and amphibians |
|
|
|
here, all the way through birds and mammals at the |
|
|
|
top are highly conserved, antagonistic circuits. |
|
|
|
What I mean by antagonistic these things don't work together. |
|
|
|
They are on or off. |
|
|
|
You can't be doing this and that. |
|
|
|
They are antagonistic in controlling either the activity of social |
|
|
|
bonding, parental behaviour, or aggressive behaviour towards offspring. |
|
|
|
And adult animals can display parental care or aggression according |
|
|
|
to their physiological state. |
|
|
|
Are they ready to do that? |
|
|
|
And what is the environment? |
|
|
|
So it's a combination of that. |
|
|
|
So these are these are statements. |
|
|
|
These are course conclusions that come from that review, and |
|
|
|
we'll dive into those in a moment. |
|
|
|
But it's worth being aware, we live in quite a |
|
|
|
civilised vertical society that if you step out of the |
|
|
|
door, you're not going to see a huge amount of |
|
|
|
aggression, I hope, today. |
|
|
|
But I have to go back more than 200 years |
|
|
|
and you would see a lot of aggression in humans |
|
|
|
and there is a lot of aggression in other parts |
|
|
|
of the world. |
|
|
|
Very sadly, there are wars going on. |
|
|
|
We are quite violent species. |
|
|
|
But that's also true of a lot of other mammals |
|
|
|
and other species. |
|
|
|
There's a lot of fighting for survival out there, and |
|
|
|
that requires that fighting and obtaining the the nutrients and |
|
|
|
the food and water you need requires aggression. |
|
|
|
You have to fight for things, for survival that Lucian |
|
|
|
has built into the brain of these mammals and other |
|
|
|
other vertebrates. |
|
|
|
Aggressive behaviour to survive. |
|
|
|
But whether they're aggressive or caring depends on the situation. |
|
|
|
So this is a graph from that paper that'll take |
|
|
|
a little while to unpack. |
|
|
|
So what we can see here and explain the y |
|
|
|
axis, first of all, this y axis, and this comes |
|
|
|
from a this review. |
|
|
|
As I said, here's some quick actions. |
|
|
|
If you look at the Y axis is what percentage |
|
|
|
of a group of male laboratory mice, these are males |
|
|
|
who are virgin as and they're not they're not mated |
|
|
|
previously. |
|
|
|
And they're previously this is this is a graph after |
|
|
|
those male cf1 strain mice have first and they're like |
|
|
|
mated. |
|
|
|
So they have mated. |
|
|
|
They're not going to have offspring. |
|
|
|
And what it shows you in the initial period before |
|
|
|
or just after that is the natural behaviour. |
|
|
|
This is what most male male mice will do, and |
|
|
|
this is quite similar to a lot of other rodent |
|
|
|
species and other mammals. |
|
|
|
Lions, for example, BE The classic example is that most |
|
|
|
of the time, given the opportunity, if that mice finds |
|
|
|
another unprotected pup in other mice, it will attack it, |
|
|
|
it'll destroy it. |
|
|
|
It's, it's trying to unless it's a pup, it won't, |
|
|
|
it will treat it with attack. |
|
|
|
It might ignore it in many cases. |
|
|
|
Occasionally it might retrieve that pup. |
|
|
|
There might be social circumstances under which it gets activated |
|
|
|
to help retrieve that problem. |
|
|
|
But you can see just in the spirit this fluctuates |
|
|
|
from here. |
|
|
|
80% of all the male mice to looking at Habitat, |
|
|
|
the men being given this experiment just a few days |
|
|
|
after mating. |
|
|
|
But then there's a radical shift in these male mice. |
|
|
|
His brain. |
|
|
|
Something is switching off that aggressive circuit, and it dropped |
|
|
|
to 10% of them are attacking and 80% of them |
|
|
|
are switching to retrieving pups. |
|
|
|
That can't be their own pups because they've just mated. |
|
|
|
But it will be other pups they find scattered in |
|
|
|
the environment. |
|
|
|
And that slowly declines and is a shift after about |
|
|
|
60 days. |
|
|
|
And the idea is that they're. |
|
|
|
Is a rapid a period where they need to avoid |
|
|
|
attacking to support the female. |
|
|
|
But after that period isn't that they will typically be |
|
|
|
left that scenario and they'll be back to their standard |
|
|
|
state and switching. |
|
|
|
So what we get out of this experiment, is it |
|
|
|
really true? |
|
|
|
Is it real physiological change induced by that process of |
|
|
|
mating? |
|
|
|
And so we can see a similar pattern to what's |
|
|
|
going on here in wild female mice. |
|
|
|
But that doesn't happen in laboratory female mice, intriguingly, that |
|
|
|
sort of they've been bred to be a bit more |
|
|
|
docile and nicer to pups. |
|
|
|
That's what ends up being used in lab experiments. |
|
|
|
They're not as aggressive. |
|
|
|
So what causes that? |
|
|
|
So this is a great grass in Davidson, could be |
|
|
|
in the wild watching this and describing it and say, |
|
|
|
Wow, look at this. |
|
|
|
Amazing behaviour is a remarkable behaviour we can see in |
|
|
|
the animal kingdom. |
|
|
|
But you're not in nature show. |
|
|
|
You're in a neurobiology lecture. |
|
|
|
Why? |
|
|
|
Why do we get this? |
|
|
|
Well, what these scientists have done their research is this |
|
|
|
their time dependent? |
|
|
|
So in that process, synaptic changes to the sign ups |
|
|
|
as there have a changing in those male mice brains |
|
|
|
and rest transcriptional. |
|
|
|
So that's within the DNA you're having different readout of |
|
|
|
the genes is a change in gene expression and triggered |
|
|
|
by that mating process. |
|
|
|
So that mating process causes the release of different chemical |
|
|
|
cues that change that. |
|
|
|
And some of them are released by females during pregnancy, |
|
|
|
can drive that radical behaviour shift from killing to parenting. |
|
|
|
What they found is if you disrupt the verbal maze |
|
|
|
Logan in wild male virgin males, it will reduce that |
|
|
|
process. |
|
|
|
If I go back to that graph here, you took |
|
|
|
a mouse, take a group of mice and you disrupt |
|
|
|
their vulnerable needs. |
|
|
|
Logan They normally would attack, but the ones we don't |
|
|
|
have the capacity to detect those cues drop down. |
|
|
|
They start behaving as if they mated. |
|
|
|
They're no longer detecting the signals that cause them to |
|
|
|
attack. |
|
|
|
So that formalised logic is not just useful for going, |
|
|
|
oh, detecting your friends or your children, but detecting strangers |
|
|
|
for the mice. |
|
|
|
Why they attacking these pups? |
|
|
|
Is it trying to preserve their own particular gene pool |
|
|
|
of their own genes? |
|
|
|
If they can make the mouse, the male mice can |
|
|
|
maybe all the other female mice and it can have |
|
|
|
its offspring grow up. |
|
|
|
The best thing it can do is kill off the |
|
|
|
other competitors children. |
|
|
|
And again, there are lots of other mammals that do |
|
|
|
this. |
|
|
|
Lions or the famous example, it will kill a new |
|
|
|
line and just the bride takes it will kill the |
|
|
|
the cubs from the other lions. |
|
|
|
That happens to be in that pride. |
|
|
|
But you also see these going to patterns and berry |
|
|
|
burying beetles. |
|
|
|
So not just vertebrates and lots of birds. |
|
|
|
The changes in the female are more varied. |
|
|
|
So you have the well known features to do with |
|
|
|
oestrogen. |
|
|
|
Progesterone and prolactin are all three hormones that circulate through |
|
|
|
the body in females during pregnancy that change that maternal |
|
|
|
behaviour in males. |
|
|
|
There's also testosterone very familiar culturally with the idea of |
|
|
|
testosterone is linked to aggression. |
|
|
|
It regulates a lot of that is very high in |
|
|
|
the males and it can be reduced in fathers to |
|
|
|
produce less aggressive behaviour. |
|
|
|
But of course this varies. |
|
|
|
I always remember all these things vary between people. |
|
|
|
That graph shows you this. |
|
|
|
There are some, you know, some animals, some of these |
|
|
|
males after after mating, they're still attacking and killing and |
|
|
|
there are some before they mated that are not doing |
|
|
|
that. |
|
|
|
So just be aware. |
|
|
|
There's a lot of variation out there too. |
|
|
|
Now, I've written on this slide down memory you do |
|
|
|
not need for the exam to memorise the layout and |
|
|
|
the interconnections of all these nuclei. |
|
|
|
That's not what this is about. |
|
|
|
This is a this is a figure four from that |
|
|
|
really great review. |
|
|
|
What you need to take away for this second module |
|
|
|
is that there are two of these circuits. |
|
|
|
One circuit is involved in parental care and one circuit. |
|
|
|
This involves an aggression, so particularly violence towards other strangers |
|
|
|
and pups. |
|
|
|
And the important point here is that these two circuits |
|
|
|
are linked. |
|
|
|
You can see the accessory olfactory bulb. |
|
|
|
That's the pathway you heard in the slide three where |
|
|
|
I talked about the vulnerable days and going to this |
|
|
|
area. |
|
|
|
And it projects into the aggressive aggression circuit. |
|
|
|
And if you damage that that circuit, it won't get |
|
|
|
turned on as much. |
|
|
|
They're not going to show as much aggression. |
|
|
|
The here we have another circuit involved in parental care |
|
|
|
that can get switched on. |
|
|
|
And instead they have they have these two interacting circuits. |
|
|
|
So they will, if one is on the axons from |
|
|
|
these brain areas, will terminate. |
|
|
|
And a lot of these other is to shut them |
|
|
|
down. |
|
|
|
They'll be negative impact and vice versa because you can't |
|
|
|
be both aggressive parenting at the same time. |
|
|
|
That's just because you just can't do the two different |
|
|
|
behaviours. |
|
|
|
So you notice to you there are a lot of |
|
|
|
other brain areas we come across. |
|
|
|
We've got here various areas in the amygdala that are |
|
|
|
important for Christmas, think parental care and we've got the |
|
|
|
prefrontal. |
|
|
|
Text. |
|
|
|
It's receiving and sending information back in. |
|
|
|
And you have again to remind you have a lecture |
|
|
|
at the end of the course and what the prefrontal |
|
|
|
cortex is doing by Professor Paul Burgess. |
|
|
|
So we come back to that. |
|
|
|
You can hold on to this for the moment that |
|
|
|
the prefrontal cortex, it shows a modulatory control in a |
|
|
|
lot of behaviour. |
|
|
|
One of them is the the exertion of parental control. |
|
|
|
So these two circuits exist and they they're antagonistic. |
|
|
|
What do we know about them? |
|
|
|
Is that the aversive circuit? |
|
|
|
First of all, that that red one is dominant. |
|
|
|
It's the one in charge most of the time in |
|
|
|
female virgin rats and male virgin rats. |
|
|
|
We talked about in lab female rats, mice and rats. |
|
|
|
The same sort of idea here that can can be |
|
|
|
quite docile. |
|
|
|
But generally that aggressive circuit is long. |
|
|
|
It will it will lead them to survive more. |
|
|
|
The aggression will save them as a as a rodent |
|
|
|
post-partum. |
|
|
|
So after mating and the desensitisation that occurs with in |
|
|
|
females, there's this hormonal neuro modulatory experience dependent factors that |
|
|
|
activate that, that other affiliated circuit that also acts to |
|
|
|
silence that that that the circuit, the one that does |
|
|
|
the aggression and avoidance. |
|
|
|
So just to highlight here it does this this is |
|
|
|
the avoidance and aggression. |
|
|
|
I've mainly talked about fighting, doing things, but this circuit |
|
|
|
is also important for just ignoring, running right, just not |
|
|
|
caring all these behaviours that again, are part of survival |
|
|
|
for for mice and rats. |
|
|
|
As part of us walks the toasting we talked about. |
|
|
|
That is really key in the initiation of that material |
|
|
|
behaviour. |
|
|
|
So we talked about if you inject these goals with |
|
|
|
oxidation, they won't thorns, but they also won't treat their |
|
|
|
children. |
|
|
|
They won't do the licking and grooming that we heard |
|
|
|
about under the stress lecture. |
|
|
|
So it really is important for oxytocin to be to |
|
|
|
be to be engaged for that. |
|
|
|
There's an area we're going to talk about next week |
|
|
|
in the detail of the ventral segmental area. |
|
|
|
We spend a lot of time talking about that and |
|
|
|
the molecules don't the mean and that's involved. |
|
|
|
It's a state just at this point, this brain areas |
|
|
|
involved in initiating this process and maintaining the behavioural maternal |
|
|
|
behaviour. |
|
|
|
So oxytocin is not shown here, but bonds on the |
|
|
|
receptors in the ventral segmental area and the ventral segmental |
|
|
|
areas you hear next week is the brain area that |
|
|
|
motivates and drives animals to do things again and again. |
|
|
|
So in this case, the drive here is that the |
|
|
|
animal is the, the, the females to really spend time |
|
|
|
investing with a the pups and be the partners. |
|
|
|
So that brain area is key for drives initiating and |
|
|
|
maintaining that behaviour. |
|
|
|
We'll come on to how it does that for all |
|
|
|
sorts of things. |
|
|
|
So people, the things we get involved in are habits, |
|
|
|
bad habits, Ferals that driven by this all depends on |
|
|
|
the situation. |
|
|
|
But beyond that there's also adrenaline and serotonin. |
|
|
|
Serotonin. |
|
|
|
These circuits are involved in maternal behaviours. |
|
|
|
Just to highlight in this picture here you have these |
|
|
|
various brain structures showing up here and these are the |
|
|
|
areas, the rough and the locus to release these two |
|
|
|
brain areas that release that involved in in regulating our |
|
|
|
attention, our arousal and our focus. |
|
|
|
These here appear to be part of that circuit that's |
|
|
|
involved in paternal care. |
|
|
|
And so these are also involved in that in that |
|
|
|
process. |
|
|
|
So just to end on, I'll talk through one experiment |
|
|
|
that just gives a really good two experiments. |
|
|
|
This one and the next one is a wrap up |
|
|
|
today's lecture. |
|
|
|
This is a figure from a news and Views article. |
|
|
|
So Rodriguez are writing up, trying to explain this experiment. |
|
|
|
But another group had done so. |
|
|
|
What they did in this experiment to understand the maternal |
|
|
|
paternal process for male and female is what's happening in |
|
|
|
their brains. |
|
|
|
And in this case they're looking at the medial pre |
|
|
|
optic area of the hypothalamus. |
|
|
|
So earlier on in this lecture, I showed you the |
|
|
|
hypothalamus and I explained this like a number of different |
|
|
|
nuclei within there. |
|
|
|
One of these is called the medial pre optic area. |
|
|
|
It's got nothing to do with optics. |
|
|
|
It just happens to be next to the optic nerve. |
|
|
|
So the medial pre optic area and after the animals |
|
|
|
have mated, you tend to get activation of neurones in |
|
|
|
this brain area. |
|
|
|
They're not as active before, but they become activated after |
|
|
|
mating this small bit of brain area. |
|
|
|
And that's what's indicated here by the red neurones. |
|
|
|
These are neurones that are sending out the highly active, |
|
|
|
the sending out transmission, That transmission of those neurones of |
|
|
|
the medial pre optic area they're arguing is driving that |
|
|
|
parental behaviour. |
|
|
|
And the reason they're finding these are critical is that |
|
|
|
they can go in and they can deplete those neurones |
|
|
|
just in that one tiny area. |
|
|
|
You barely see it in the brain. |
|
|
|
They've gone in and selectively damage those neurones by clever |
|
|
|
chemical techniques. |
|
|
|
But after that process you get no parental behaviour from |
|
|
|
the female or the male remaining looking at female mice |
|
|
|
and experienced male mice and experienced female mice. |
|
|
|
Like I mentioned, it's the virgin males where you don't |
|
|
|
get this pattern right. |
|
|
|
These all show this, this. |
|
|
|
If you damage this, you don't get this parental type |
|
|
|
behaviours. |
|
|
|
So that's the grooming, the retrieving pups and taking care |
|
|
|
of them. |
|
|
|
Okay, that's, that's one experiment to show it's important. |
|
|
|
That's one way of doing it. |
|
|
|
But ask these these four groups, virgin females, experienced males |
|
|
|
who mated and experienced females and mated experience here is |
|
|
|
little about mating on the right hand side. |
|
|
|
Now, this is for me. |
|
|
|
This is why this paper was published in science. |
|
|
|
That first experiments. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
It damaged the brain. |
|
|
|
They can't do it. |
|
|
|
That's fine. |
|
|
|
Very long. |
|
|
|
Here is the kicker. |
|
|
|
Experiment around. |
|
|
|
From a scientific perspective, it was virgin male mice. |
|
|
|
First of all, you can see they're not active. |
|
|
|
These cells that are active in these all three other |
|
|
|
categories of mice in the virgin males, they've not mated. |
|
|
|
They're just like young, aggressive male mice who have not |
|
|
|
made it yet. |
|
|
|
And what do they show? |
|
|
|
Aggression. |
|
|
|
They attack. |
|
|
|
They tap upset. |
|
|
|
They get rid of them. |
|
|
|
Here's what they did. |
|
|
|
They went in and artificially using optogenetics, find those cells |
|
|
|
that could be activated, activating and activate those cells by |
|
|
|
shining the laser light onto the genetically tagged cells. |
|
|
|
So what you can see is before and after that |
|
|
|
light is this this is a couple of milliseconds. |
|
|
|
You've got a normal functioning male. |
|
|
|
You shine a light. |
|
|
|
2 milliseconds later, a whole of the cells are turned |
|
|
|
on. |
|
|
|
What does it do? |
|
|
|
So rather than running across the cage and attacking and |
|
|
|
possibly trying to bite and destroy pop, it runs over, |
|
|
|
it picks it up, puts it back in the nest. |
|
|
|
So by just turning on a light, affecting a small |
|
|
|
number of cells, more than the number of neurones in |
|
|
|
this illustration here, but still you're talking about tiny cell |
|
|
|
nucleus in a small part of the brain. |
|
|
|
You switched one animal from showing this aggressive behaviour to |
|
|
|
a whole different approach. |
|
|
|
And it's really worth what's quite amazing about this is |
|
|
|
it's not like you've improved motor function or its ability |
|
|
|
to perceive light in some way. |
|
|
|
You've changed the radical feature of its entire behaviour in |
|
|
|
a millisecond. |
|
|
|
You've with this experiment, they've turned to mice that would |
|
|
|
attack and kill. |
|
|
|
It's one artificially thinks it's mated effectively. |
|
|
|
It's not thinking about this, but you've artificially activated a |
|
|
|
parental circuit in an animal that's never undergone any change. |
|
|
|
So this shows the power of the kind of optogenetics |
|
|
|
approach of exploring behaviour. |
|
|
|
So you got to ask, why did you use the |
|
|
|
light? |
|
|
|
How do you see through this activity? |
|
|
|
Yes. |
|
|
|
So optogenetics, as you heard in your lecture on the |
|
|
|
methods lecture, remember that the methods, the techniques you learned |
|
|
|
about what you could do is genetically tagged those cells |
|
|
|
in these particular rats. |
|
|
|
I think it is for mice in mice more likely |
|
|
|
get mice. |
|
|
|
They've gone and bred these animals with specific proteins that |
|
|
|
are sensitive to the wavelengths of light. |
|
|
|
When those wavelengths of light are show shot, shine down |
|
|
|
on those cells through a cut in the top of |
|
|
|
the brain of the rat, the mouse, they can artificially |
|
|
|
turn them on and they can do it within a |
|
|
|
couple of milliseconds. |
|
|
|
And so you can have a rat, a mouse running |
|
|
|
across the cage to attack and switch the behaviour in |
|
|
|
an instant from attack to being attacking an aggressive and |
|
|
|
killing to being attacked in milliseconds through one circuit interruption. |
|
|
|
So what does that what makes that amazing is that |
|
|
|
it's quite a complex behaviour you're looking at. |
|
|
|
Now this is a experiment where they were looking at |
|
|
|
another particular nucleus. |
|
|
|
The this particular part, the AVP haven't read that, but |
|
|
|
it's in this paper. |
|
|
|
So this is a hypothalamic nucleus which differs between Virgin. |
|
|
|
So a difference between males and females in virgins. |
|
|
|
But what they showed is that this particular is not |
|
|
|
just the nucleus involved with the particular subcategories of neurones |
|
|
|
that can start to get to that. |
|
|
|
You can regulate in the parents, the females. |
|
|
|
So males after mating don't show a change here, but |
|
|
|
the female mice can show a change in the number |
|
|
|
of active neurones in this particular nucleus after mating. |
|
|
|
So not only do you get these patterns of expression, |
|
|
|
but you can get them in particular male or female |
|
|
|
situations as well. |
|
|
|
But it's worth going to watch these movies to see |
|
|
|
the patterns of behaviour. |
|
|
|
So in this link, if you don't follow it through, |
|
|
|
you will see the use of optogenetics where you get |
|
|
|
mice, which is not really fighting with each other, and |
|
|
|
then they'll turn on these these neurones artificially and they |
|
|
|
can do it in males as well. |
|
|
|
So they can turn on the mouse. |
|
|
|
Yes. |
|
|
|
This is almost certain case. |
|
|
|
Oh, yes. |
|
|
|
But this is all about. |
|
|
|
So if they might be right, they might not get |
|
|
|
children from mice. |
|
|
|
It's extremely likely their breeding capacity is way higher. |
|
|
|
That's one of the reasons they've ended up being laboratory |
|
|
|
animals that are really higher breeding. |
|
|
|
They require fast turnover. |
|
|
|
They can have children very quickly, but very likely they |
|
|
|
would, but it's not dependent. |
|
|
|
So these activation patterns are not dependent on the sex |
|
|
|
success. |
|
|
|
Well, I think you would need to activate pregnancy, though, |
|
|
|
so if they don't end up being pregnant. |
|
|
|
But I think the idea of the meeting alone will |
|
|
|
activate patterns of change in the male, because they can't |
|
|
|
detect that necessarily. |
|
|
|
But to pick up on that point, when the female |
|
|
|
does become pregnant, her pheromones secretions, what she's secreting, will |
|
|
|
change. |
|
|
|
And that provides a cue because humans, we don't really |
|
|
|
dissect these things but might do dogs do. |
|
|
|
They can detect other animals can smell these things. |
|
|
|
We can't in the same way those they go and |
|
|
|
watch these movies because you can see them turning off |
|
|
|
again with the optogenetics and the midpoints to attack, making |
|
|
|
an animal docile and parent and pick up a tree |
|
|
|
pups. |
|
|
|
It's it's really one of the most dramatic pieces of |
|
|
|
evidence I've seen. |
|
|
|
And again, one of the reasons it's published in the |
|
|
|
most prestigious journal in the world in Nature. |
|
|
|
So to wrap up today, there's a nice section in |
|
|
|
the physiology of behaviour that calls in or brisket on |
|
|
|
social bonding. |
|
|
|
That review is really clear and really nice to read |
|
|
|
into and Young. |
|
|
|
I talked about the voles again, extremely easy to read |
|
|
|
article and then there's some other reading. |
|
|
|
If you're really interested you could look into This is |
|
|
|
not essential, but we can go into that and we'll |
|
|
|
see you next week. |
|
|
|
The consciousness and motivation, why we do the things we |
|
|
|
do. |