|
Good morning. |
|
|
|
Just checking the mic works. |
|
|
|
Yeah. |
|
|
|
You can hear me. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
Okay. |
|
|
|
We're going to get going with our lecture now. |
|
|
|
It's 5 minutes past the hour. |
|
|
|
So welcome to our first lecture in brain and behaviour. |
|
|
|
Probably your first lecture, this academic term for these lectures. |
|
|
|
We're in this chemistry auditorium, and it's very difficult as |
|
|
|
you find, to come in and out. |
|
|
|
So we will make some efforts to create a path |
|
|
|
for the students. |
|
|
|
It is very exciting to be back lecturing you. |
|
|
|
I hope you were excited to be back in person. |
|
|
|
We've got 20 of these lectures of this course. |
|
|
|
I'm Professor Hugo Speers. |
|
|
|
It's my details, it's my email address, my lab and |
|
|
|
the title of today's lecture was very much looking forward |
|
|
|
to teaching you on Moodle. |
|
|
|
I'll just show you the the team briefly as myself |
|
|
|
as Sam Solomon, who's a professor in psychology, a new |
|
|
|
lecturer, Professor Stephen Fleming, Professor Daniel Bender, Paul Burgess, who's |
|
|
|
mysteriously Hidden Valley Academy, who will teach you research methods. |
|
|
|
And amongst them we have a team of experts who |
|
|
|
are coming in to teach you today, and I'm the |
|
|
|
first one up and I run. |
|
|
|
I'm the convenor for this module. |
|
|
|
All the lectures will be lecture cost. |
|
|
|
If something goes wrong with those, we have an entire |
|
|
|
back catalogue of carefully recorded lectures that are almost identical |
|
|
|
to these. |
|
|
|
So please do attend. |
|
|
|
We hope the idea of these lectures that are starting |
|
|
|
today are to kind of be there in person to |
|
|
|
speak to you, hopefully inspire you to go and read |
|
|
|
the things and have a voice, someone you're actually physically |
|
|
|
talking to you. |
|
|
|
So without further ado, I'll head into my first slide. |
|
|
|
So human brain, there's a picture of the human brain. |
|
|
|
There's this sequence of 20 lectures. |
|
|
|
A brain in behaviour is all about this one object. |
|
|
|
It's by far the most complicated object in the entire |
|
|
|
universe, human brain. |
|
|
|
It's capable of all the people being here. |
|
|
|
Me talking to you now, all the discoveries, all the |
|
|
|
history, everything humanity has ever done in terms of what |
|
|
|
it's produced has come from the faculties of this brain |
|
|
|
over other species. |
|
|
|
It's an absolutely remarkable thing. |
|
|
|
The beginning of today's lecture on our 20 lectures diving |
|
|
|
into topics in brain of behaviour. |
|
|
|
It's to take a step back and think, Wow, sitting |
|
|
|
inside your head, behind your eyes, looking at me. |
|
|
|
Now, next between your ears, listening to me now is |
|
|
|
this object. |
|
|
|
It looks quite attractive in this well, relatively in this |
|
|
|
picture. |
|
|
|
But if you were to see a real brain, if |
|
|
|
someone was to have it, you know, accidentally removed in |
|
|
|
front of us, it would be disgusting. |
|
|
|
It's full of blood. |
|
|
|
It's vastly for the blood vessels. |
|
|
|
It's a really squidgy, pretty disgusting thing. |
|
|
|
And for thousands of years in our prehistory millennia, people |
|
|
|
weren't very aware of what it was doing. |
|
|
|
So today's journey is let's take a look at the |
|
|
|
brain history of the scientists. |
|
|
|
This study, that is a way. |
|
|
|
And if you do get into the brain and slice |
|
|
|
it up, as I said, pretty disgusting. |
|
|
|
You don't see very much. |
|
|
|
The brain is just like this dull kind of, you |
|
|
|
know, beige colour. |
|
|
|
It's more disgusting than not when it's fresh. |
|
|
|
But the scientists discovered over 200 years ago, if you |
|
|
|
take something like silver nitrate, you can make dark stains |
|
|
|
happen in stuff. |
|
|
|
Most of the time you don't want to stain things, |
|
|
|
but if you're looking down a microscope that's been developed |
|
|
|
and you put a stain into a brain section, an |
|
|
|
entire world opens to your eyes the early microscope, people |
|
|
|
using microscopes. |
|
|
|
There's early microscopist looking down with stain sections, discovered an |
|
|
|
amazing world. |
|
|
|
Here's a single brain cell in one and one of |
|
|
|
these early sections. |
|
|
|
If you look even closer, you can see these brain |
|
|
|
cells take on these weird, strange forms. |
|
|
|
Now, I'm a biased scientist. |
|
|
|
I find these B cells that are just quite dramatic |
|
|
|
to look at. |
|
|
|
But there's not just individual cells. |
|
|
|
There's whole collections of networks of these cells. |
|
|
|
Here's some examples. |
|
|
|
They're they're arranged in the most beautiful ways inside your |
|
|
|
head as you're listening to me in how carefully arranged |
|
|
|
neurones are doing important work of allowing you to understand |
|
|
|
what I'm saying. |
|
|
|
Take notes, think about what I'm saying as well. |
|
|
|
If we cut through on the lowest bit, is your |
|
|
|
body behind your neck and you look into the spinal |
|
|
|
cord, you will see this object. |
|
|
|
This is a cut through the spinal cord. |
|
|
|
You could cut through a bat, a rat, a durable, |
|
|
|
a human. |
|
|
|
And it would very similar to this. |
|
|
|
And again, the early this is hundreds of years old, |
|
|
|
this picture drawn by these early anatomists, Peking duck microscopes. |
|
|
|
If you look really close, these things look a bit |
|
|
|
like sort of strange alien creatures with these incredible fibres, |
|
|
|
connections, nerve cells are the neurones are very complicated and |
|
|
|
take all sorts of forms and shapes and they're quite |
|
|
|
incredible pictures to look at to start your journey into |
|
|
|
brain in behaviour, the neurones you're going to be looking |
|
|
|
at in a lot of the pictures of the science |
|
|
|
today are quite boring. |
|
|
|
We've reduced them down to simplicity. |
|
|
|
I won't be showing exciting pictures like this through the |
|
|
|
rest of the lectures, but I just want to start |
|
|
|
with the beauty that exists in the topic we're looking |
|
|
|
at. |
|
|
|
Here's a particularly beautiful drawing one individual cell and a |
|
|
|
sort of climbing block network of fibres from another cell |
|
|
|
winding their way round that cell. |
|
|
|
And all of those drawings I've just taken you through |
|
|
|
step by step were drawn by one anatomist, Santiago Ramon |
|
|
|
Cajal, the Spanish Year, an optimist who won the Nobel |
|
|
|
Prize. |
|
|
|
That discovery, he was the person who who argued. |
|
|
|
First of all, I think they're brain cells. |
|
|
|
It was a controversial idea. |
|
|
|
There were brain cells and we'll come onto that in |
|
|
|
a moment. |
|
|
|
Reproduced all those beautiful images. |
|
|
|
It was arrested in Oxford, actually, for trying to draw |
|
|
|
pictures of the local community buildings because they just couldn't |
|
|
|
believe he was really a scientist travelling Oxford. |
|
|
|
They put him in jail, had to be let out |
|
|
|
of jail. |
|
|
|
But he's written incredible books about his life experience winning |
|
|
|
the Nobel Prize and what you need to do as |
|
|
|
a scientist. |
|
|
|
Come back to him before we come back to the |
|
|
|
scientists that made the stories behind this, I just want |
|
|
|
to highlight you. |
|
|
|
You are UCL student sitting in one of the world's |
|
|
|
top universities for this topic. |
|
|
|
We are kind of the top research institute in the |
|
|
|
whole of Europe for neuroscience and the topics we're talking |
|
|
|
about today. |
|
|
|
That means excessive amounts of money coming into UCL around |
|
|
|
this excessive amount of scientists have gone in, a number |
|
|
|
of Nobel prizes have been made here. |
|
|
|
There are lots of fellows the Royal Society in this |
|
|
|
topic. |
|
|
|
The story keeps going on. |
|
|
|
So we're ranked second in the world was slightly behind |
|
|
|
Harvard and M.I.T. If you put those two together, they |
|
|
|
have a slightly higher number of Nobel Prize winners and |
|
|
|
money have a lot more money. |
|
|
|
Anyway. |
|
|
|
What are we here to learn? |
|
|
|
So every lecture we're going to get, you try and |
|
|
|
get what are the objectives? |
|
|
|
So today's objective of this lecture are two things I |
|
|
|
want you to come away with. |
|
|
|
Once you acquire the key and knowledge of the key |
|
|
|
developments in our historical understanding of brain and behaviour. |
|
|
|
My lectures in this course in the others are not |
|
|
|
just about the structure of the brain for its own |
|
|
|
sake, or how so receptors work. |
|
|
|
We're absolutely obsessed in this course by behaviour. |
|
|
|
This is the links between how the brain gives rise |
|
|
|
to behaviour. |
|
|
|
We also want you in this course to learn about |
|
|
|
some of the key historical figures. |
|
|
|
So if you were to pass in some wisdom, who |
|
|
|
was who? |
|
|
|
Who developed the ideas behind the brain? |
|
|
|
So you would have an idea of who these celebrities |
|
|
|
are that would run through maybe a few snippets of |
|
|
|
facts about. |
|
|
|
When you come to sit exams, the course will kind |
|
|
|
of throw up and things. |
|
|
|
So right from the get go, I want you to |
|
|
|
think thinking about if we were to put up an |
|
|
|
exam question after this lecture, what's the kind of exam |
|
|
|
question we want to ask? |
|
|
|
And you can put that in your head what you're |
|
|
|
thinking through doing, How would I get to that? |
|
|
|
And it might be something like how has her understanding |
|
|
|
of the brain changed since the ancient times? |
|
|
|
So what did the ancient people know and what do |
|
|
|
we do now? |
|
|
|
And I'll be teaching that in this lecture today. |
|
|
|
We also run an MQ multiple choice exam for this |
|
|
|
lecture, which is extremely easy last year, where we improving |
|
|
|
that a little bit, but we'll be giving you lots |
|
|
|
of help to make sure that you're absolutely up to |
|
|
|
speed with the MQ. |
|
|
|
I'm sure you'll nail it as long as you're students |
|
|
|
that. |
|
|
|
So an example question might be which ancient culture first |
|
|
|
is first thought to have a specific word for brain |
|
|
|
in the written text? |
|
|
|
Greeks, Babylonians, Egyptians. |
|
|
|
Persians. |
|
|
|
If you go down to the British Museum and ask |
|
|
|
the answer to that question, you'll find out in a |
|
|
|
moment and give The answer is the ancient Egyptians. |
|
|
|
So it's example of a kind of level of question |
|
|
|
will be asking. |
|
|
|
Let's start right back. |
|
|
|
Not with the ancient Egyptians. |
|
|
|
Let's go far back in time before those giant pyramids |
|
|
|
were built and racing was even invented back to skulls |
|
|
|
around 7000 years ago. |
|
|
|
This one is it pretty looks like a sort of |
|
|
|
Halloween type of skull. |
|
|
|
It's quite intense. |
|
|
|
But there's a key feature I'm sure you've all noticed |
|
|
|
about this skull as a cut mark in the top |
|
|
|
of his head. |
|
|
|
A lot, a lot of historical skull was dug up |
|
|
|
in 7000 years ago, have holes in them through axes, |
|
|
|
spears in a brutal life 7000 years ago. |
|
|
|
What's really creepy about this one is that the bone |
|
|
|
had been cut and healed. |
|
|
|
This person walked around living with this whole cold, carefully |
|
|
|
cart, not cut with an axe or something in their |
|
|
|
head. |
|
|
|
And so scientists as well, archaeologists and palaeontologists have argued |
|
|
|
that this cutting into the head happening was found 7000 |
|
|
|
years ago. |
|
|
|
And we think we don't know. |
|
|
|
There's no written text, but it was used for medical |
|
|
|
treatment. |
|
|
|
Perhaps this person had epilepsy or some other condition they |
|
|
|
would. |
|
|
|
They thought maybe if we open up the skull we'll |
|
|
|
get the demons out. |
|
|
|
What's interesting about this is that they thought that cutting |
|
|
|
into the skull, into the head may be doing something |
|
|
|
we don't fully understand, that we have no real way |
|
|
|
of knowing. |
|
|
|
But this is the earliest kind of thinking about humans |
|
|
|
might be aware of the brain and doing something to |
|
|
|
to medically approach it. |
|
|
|
So we come now three and 4000 years forwards. |
|
|
|
When the great Pyramids were built in ancient Egypt and |
|
|
|
this incredible moment in civilisation when people were writing out |
|
|
|
these hieroglyphic texts, which until, you know, a couple hundred |
|
|
|
years ago were indecipherable and suddenly through the Rosetta Stone |
|
|
|
in a British museum, they were able to decipher and |
|
|
|
work out what was written in this hieroglyphics. |
|
|
|
And what they discovered was that in these ancient texts |
|
|
|
they were able to find from ancient burial sites was |
|
|
|
that the ancient Egyptians, in fact, did write about treatments |
|
|
|
for brain damage. |
|
|
|
What to do if you're a medical doctor and someone |
|
|
|
has damage to the head. |
|
|
|
These are some of the things one might do. |
|
|
|
Critically impressive things have been 5000 years ago it was |
|
|
|
the ancient Egyptians discover that if someone hits like a |
|
|
|
spear or some axe hits the right side of your |
|
|
|
head, you lose the ability to move with the left |
|
|
|
side of your body. |
|
|
|
And if you smash into the left side of the |
|
|
|
head, you'll lose the ability to move or say on |
|
|
|
the right side of your body. |
|
|
|
So this is known as this contralateral movement sensation. |
|
|
|
So when I say contralateral, we'll get into that on |
|
|
|
Friday's lecture about what we mean by these anatomical terms |
|
|
|
that are quite remarkable. |
|
|
|
This is sitting unknown for thousands of years in transcripts, |
|
|
|
and it critically had this word brain. |
|
|
|
You can see the hieroglyphics of the word brain. |
|
|
|
But one fact is that they when they mummified the |
|
|
|
ancient Egyptian bodies, most of you be aware of this, |
|
|
|
they would put them into these tombs. |
|
|
|
It's hard not to know about this, but they obviously |
|
|
|
save parts of the body and put them into these |
|
|
|
special jars like the intestines and the heart, particularly the |
|
|
|
heart. |
|
|
|
I just threw away the brain. |
|
|
|
I thought it was not something to be preserved into |
|
|
|
the afterlife. |
|
|
|
So they clearly had some insights. |
|
|
|
But then the cultural practice of burial didn't really follow |
|
|
|
through. |
|
|
|
We now move forward. |
|
|
|
We're going to go through individual people. |
|
|
|
These are the first person hypocrisies we'll go through with |
|
|
|
all the way through it. |
|
|
|
A lot of bearded men, a lot of white men |
|
|
|
will eventually end on a female scientist passing through a |
|
|
|
critical Persian scientist. |
|
|
|
So we'll begin with this chap. |
|
|
|
Hypocrisies. |
|
|
|
Now he is the first person who people wrote about |
|
|
|
mentioning his his followers wrote about him, saying, Here's all |
|
|
|
the incredible things, hypocrisies. |
|
|
|
We know when he lived, when he died. |
|
|
|
We can date now. |
|
|
|
We have evidence of his existence. |
|
|
|
And why he's up here is that he is the |
|
|
|
first person named, we can say, as far as we're |
|
|
|
aware, to argue that the brain that was what we're |
|
|
|
going to teach you in this course. |
|
|
|
Brain supports the mind, supports your behaviour. |
|
|
|
He argued this because he's he he was he was |
|
|
|
really argues to be the first physician, the first doctor |
|
|
|
who went round gathering information, building on knowledge and systematising |
|
|
|
it to treat people so that doctors will take the |
|
|
|
Hippocratic oath. |
|
|
|
Do no harm to my patients. |
|
|
|
You know, you might be treating someone you really don't |
|
|
|
like, but as a Hippocratic Oath, you treat them nonetheless. |
|
|
|
And this is where the Hippocratic Oath comes from. |
|
|
|
Hypocrisy is arguing medicine should be for treatment above all |
|
|
|
other things. |
|
|
|
Critically, for today, he stands out as the person who |
|
|
|
argued that your behaviour, all the movements you're making are |
|
|
|
fundamentally coming from systems in your brain. |
|
|
|
But he didn't know how. |
|
|
|
He couldn't get much into it beyond that cool fact. |
|
|
|
And that was a great advance. |
|
|
|
It was held back by two very, very clever people. |
|
|
|
This is Plato and Aristotle who came after hypocrisies. |
|
|
|
This is the painting Leonardo da Vinci in the Vatican, |
|
|
|
and we don't have any current pictures of them from |
|
|
|
the time. |
|
|
|
So they were they were philosophers that argued in and |
|
|
|
really thinking about things, using your mind to mull over |
|
|
|
and weigh up the natural philosophy to get into what |
|
|
|
the state of the world really is. |
|
|
|
They weren't so obsessed with doing experiments, looking at things |
|
|
|
that came much later. |
|
|
|
What they argue. |
|
|
|
Plato in particular argued that reason in perception was in |
|
|
|
the head. |
|
|
|
So we do have this kind of sense from Plato |
|
|
|
that some of the head might not be the brain |
|
|
|
is this may not be the brain tissue itself, it |
|
|
|
could be something else is what we might perceive the |
|
|
|
world. |
|
|
|
But all the passions, all your emotions, everything that drives |
|
|
|
you, it's not to do with your brain. |
|
|
|
It's in your heart. |
|
|
|
We still have the idea of like, you know, we |
|
|
|
do these heart emojis on your phone. |
|
|
|
You will send hearts to say you care about somebody. |
|
|
|
This goes back to those early ancient Greek times. |
|
|
|
Aristotle even argued further, and he said he didn't think |
|
|
|
that the brain was in charge of the body's movements |
|
|
|
and the heart and foot. |
|
|
|
And Aristotle actually went and did dissections. |
|
|
|
He would get into lakes and take out fish and |
|
|
|
cut into the fish and see what happens if you |
|
|
|
cut into a brain, the animal, the main nervous system, |
|
|
|
the had, you can still see a lot of movement |
|
|
|
from the animal. |
|
|
|
Its spine will still carry on doing things. |
|
|
|
But if you cut it and stop the animal's heart, |
|
|
|
the animal will die. |
|
|
|
Stop doing any movement. |
|
|
|
And Aristotle thought this is better evidence. |
|
|
|
I can also see through Aristotle with his eye. |
|
|
|
The blood vessels go everywhere. |
|
|
|
The blood vessels permeate everything. |
|
|
|
In this living being, the heart must be the sight |
|
|
|
which controls that he couldn't see in the detail. |
|
|
|
The nerve fibres that we know now exist or what |
|
|
|
they were doing. |
|
|
|
This really held up for a long time thinking about |
|
|
|
threatening behaviour, and it persisted into the, you know, the |
|
|
|
Middle Ages because these two men, Plato and Aristotle, had |
|
|
|
so many insights into philosophy about ethics and the nature |
|
|
|
of reality, and they were held up as absolute giants. |
|
|
|
Disagreeing with them is hard, but as we go down, |
|
|
|
we've gone through the first two characters. |
|
|
|
When I moved to Galen. |
|
|
|
It's not until we get to ancient Rome and the |
|
|
|
gladiatorial combats that occurred there that delivered a huge number |
|
|
|
of hideously mangled bodies. |
|
|
|
The brutality of ancient Rome in early, you know, 150 |
|
|
|
A.D. was not a nice place, perhaps unless you were |
|
|
|
an emperor. |
|
|
|
Even that as an emperor likely to be killed by |
|
|
|
your brothers. |
|
|
|
But this this man, Galen, was a Greek physician who |
|
|
|
moved to Rome, who operated there, was looking at these |
|
|
|
cases and saying he found all these bodies and found |
|
|
|
out the effects of different bits of damage to the |
|
|
|
brain was happening and took penne and produced a cheese |
|
|
|
like an argument written down of prescriptions. |
|
|
|
The doctors that were preserved in scrolls that were handed |
|
|
|
down about the nature of physiology. |
|
|
|
And he made those of these not if you're in |
|
|
|
a medical school you historically be learning about guy with |
|
|
|
lots of reasons today I'm teaching you to he argued |
|
|
|
back to evocative was right you the a gladiator comes |
|
|
|
in with half his head in this thing he will |
|
|
|
lose all sorts of functions. |
|
|
|
So we argued against Aristotle. |
|
|
|
The brain is responsible for sensation of behaviour. |
|
|
|
Now. |
|
|
|
Galen was doing physiology, looking at black bodies coming in. |
|
|
|
He was thinking about what does this do? |
|
|
|
What if we go to ancient Persia, becomes a Powhatan |
|
|
|
or Ohaneze and is the Latinised name for this scholar? |
|
|
|
So the ancient rabbit country was amazing. |
|
|
|
A number of advances they made in thinking. |
|
|
|
So it's not just this incredible scientist was not just |
|
|
|
about saying, Oh, I can see this. |
|
|
|
This body, this brain is important for moving. |
|
|
|
He started you can see on the right side there |
|
|
|
one of his diagrams. |
|
|
|
What you see is hard to make out in that. |
|
|
|
But the bottom bit of that curly bit is I'll |
|
|
|
get a pointer if I can find. |
|
|
|
Oh, no, not at this point. |
|
|
|
The bottom part here is the nose and here are |
|
|
|
the eyeballs and here are the the trucks, the infant |
|
|
|
and the nerve fibres that come out of the eyeballs |
|
|
|
and go into the back of the brain. |
|
|
|
Now, the current thinking back in, you know, we jump |
|
|
|
forward quite a bit and it's it's sort of 900 |
|
|
|
plus A.D. hundreds of years later. |
|
|
|
Host Galen the still the thinking was that a bit |
|
|
|
like I can touch the world around me I send |
|
|
|
out and I touch this this theatre with my this |
|
|
|
lecturer with my fingers Our eyes are doing something like |
|
|
|
that with the visual world. |
|
|
|
I'm kind of prodding the world with my eyes. |
|
|
|
They're actively sensing out there and our hands and said, |
|
|
|
No, I do not think this is what's going on |
|
|
|
because he was obsessed with optics and the mathematics of |
|
|
|
optics. |
|
|
|
So you could see the eye was exactly like some |
|
|
|
of the incredible optics. |
|
|
|
And he wrote, These strategic treaties is arguing that our |
|
|
|
brain is absorbing the light from the world. |
|
|
|
Like the light coming into my eyes, I can see |
|
|
|
because I'm processing the light into the back of the |
|
|
|
brain. |
|
|
|
He didn't even think it was in the eyes. |
|
|
|
Incredible advances from our hands. |
|
|
|
And about that is a fantastic YouTube video to watch |
|
|
|
about his historical discoveries. |
|
|
|
We'll jump forward now, maybe another 500 years until there's |
|
|
|
a next big advance in terms of brain behaviour. |
|
|
|
Reach up to Renaissance Italy. |
|
|
|
This is the time of the Medici and a brutal |
|
|
|
life again. |
|
|
|
This is a frequently picking this up in the coming |
|
|
|
to Italy, but medieval there is a renaissance occurring which |
|
|
|
is incredible science, incredible odds. |
|
|
|
And in this one man, Andreas Vesalius we have both. |
|
|
|
We have incredible scientists. |
|
|
|
He was a polymath. |
|
|
|
It had lots of different things, highly liked, very charismatic |
|
|
|
to it rhymes. |
|
|
|
Crucially, we're now in 1500s. |
|
|
|
He was able to print what he was doing. |
|
|
|
He's able to take, as we can see in this |
|
|
|
diagram here, this is one really beautiful drawings, although very |
|
|
|
disgusting. |
|
|
|
This isn't a human man's head because he's nose with |
|
|
|
a skin removed, the skull removed in the brain, much |
|
|
|
less nice than the first image of a brain. |
|
|
|
But he had labelled it all. |
|
|
|
He provided the first book published back in 1500s. |
|
|
|
The Doumani Corporis Fabrica structure, The fabric of the human |
|
|
|
body, covered everything in real detail so that any anatomist |
|
|
|
in the world, anyone teaching medical school could go through. |
|
|
|
And now this time we have the brain really in |
|
|
|
detail. |
|
|
|
Consider what bits might we have in the brain. |
|
|
|
He also Andreas Vesalius started this process of thinking about |
|
|
|
the maybe the soul, our mind. |
|
|
|
This idea kind of interferes with our actions through the |
|
|
|
fluid that's in your brain is a milky, pretty disgusting |
|
|
|
substance in your head. |
|
|
|
I'll talk to you about it next Friday. |
|
|
|
When we come back, the lecture to these ventricles, these |
|
|
|
holes inside your head look absolutely swollen. |
|
|
|
If you have hydrocephalus, it's a very unfortunate condition by |
|
|
|
the brain. |
|
|
|
If you see someone hydrocephalus, the skull expands the ventricle. |
|
|
|
But he thought this was particularly the way in which |
|
|
|
our our mind interacts with our body and is a |
|
|
|
very charismatic character. |
|
|
|
This idea was really picked up in detail about this |
|
|
|
very famous philosopher, René Descartes. |
|
|
|
So if you had two lectures in philosophy, you would |
|
|
|
have heard a lot of our stuff in Plato. |
|
|
|
You then hear a lot about René Descartes is famous |
|
|
|
for arguing, I think for Rico or ergo, some just |
|
|
|
famous statements that What can we really know about reality? |
|
|
|
If I'm thinking it must be there. |
|
|
|
Today's lecture What's important about René Descartes is that he |
|
|
|
argued that there's a big distinction, that there are some |
|
|
|
things that I can't If I put my hand in |
|
|
|
a fire, I got no choice. |
|
|
|
I have to take it out. |
|
|
|
I could try and hold my hand in there, but |
|
|
|
naturally I'll take it out. |
|
|
|
And we share that with all other animals. |
|
|
|
What he had, what he. |
|
|
|
We have reflexes, things that help us survive without thinking. |
|
|
|
But there are other times when I might want to |
|
|
|
get up and slap someone. |
|
|
|
And I have decided I'm going to do I'm going |
|
|
|
to make this decision. |
|
|
|
I'm going to use my arm. |
|
|
|
I'm not going to take it. |
|
|
|
I'm going to take and slap someone. |
|
|
|
And he drew diagrams highlighting. |
|
|
|
We can see one here about the bottom that you |
|
|
|
put your foot in the fire. |
|
|
|
You'll need to take it out quickly. |
|
|
|
But the other drawing at the top is about the |
|
|
|
idea. |
|
|
|
He started to think about sensation through to action, that |
|
|
|
I might see something in the world. |
|
|
|
It passes into my brain and then I can think |
|
|
|
and then move my hand to change the world. |
|
|
|
Before René Descartes, that wasn't really a distinction. |
|
|
|
It was drawn this idea about the process, the science |
|
|
|
of what might be going on. |
|
|
|
And that's what we'll be obsessing over in the lectures |
|
|
|
going forward into a whole range of why is it |
|
|
|
exciting topics about emotions, movement, seeing, acting, smelling and memory. |
|
|
|
All these things are going on inside our head. |
|
|
|
But René Descartes really was the first to argue that, |
|
|
|
you know, he discovered all sorts of ideas in mathematics. |
|
|
|
The fact we have graphs, you know, algebra, a vast |
|
|
|
range of things. |
|
|
|
But he was really critical. |
|
|
|
He argues that the soul, this mysterious thing that we |
|
|
|
can't quite tangibly understand is separate from my body. |
|
|
|
The thinking behind me is separate. |
|
|
|
Going back to the long historical is not the way |
|
|
|
science is. |
|
|
|
Good. |
|
|
|
Now think. |
|
|
|
But you will be getting a lecture from an expert |
|
|
|
on consciousness in this course. |
|
|
|
We'll take you into some of these ideas where we |
|
|
|
are now in 2022. |
|
|
|
And if you jump forward into the 100 years after |
|
|
|
René Descartes got interested in reflexes and how he might |
|
|
|
act and the mechanics of moving, we have this again, |
|
|
|
a really charismatic character. |
|
|
|
Once again, Italian themes from 1700s. |
|
|
|
This is Luigi. |
|
|
|
Galvani is primarily a physicist, is messing around with all |
|
|
|
sorts of things in physics, but he also got obsessed |
|
|
|
with physiology and other things. |
|
|
|
A lot of these people back then didn't have to |
|
|
|
spend three years in a psychology degree to get to |
|
|
|
grips with what was going on there. |
|
|
|
He was able to learn a lot and discover things. |
|
|
|
What he built was a vast room like this. |
|
|
|
I five from here to the wall over there, vast |
|
|
|
room filled with copper plates and Wolff are huge, big |
|
|
|
thing and copper plates and wool all stacked up because |
|
|
|
he discovered, you know, if you accidentally move on a |
|
|
|
carpet, if you're wearing the wrong shoes, you can get |
|
|
|
this nasty electrical charge this well, let's make loads of |
|
|
|
that. |
|
|
|
Let's put a lot of this kind of nasty electrical |
|
|
|
shock together, put it what we can see here, put |
|
|
|
it together is this huge, big building connects all those |
|
|
|
copper plates up in and out and to a wire. |
|
|
|
I can take a little while now and I can |
|
|
|
tap it onto another wire and make a little flush |
|
|
|
of electricity. |
|
|
|
And what he discovered when he did that was he |
|
|
|
was testing out what he could do with this. |
|
|
|
And he got, you know, some frogs legs might have |
|
|
|
been part of a meal back then, but he was |
|
|
|
stimulating the frogs in different bits and finds, you know, |
|
|
|
he stimulated blood vessels. |
|
|
|
Nothing very boring. |
|
|
|
But if you stimulate the top of his legs on |
|
|
|
the white bit, the nerve fibres bundle the frogs legs |
|
|
|
on their own, will show the exact pattern of movement |
|
|
|
the frog would make, the whole frog would make trying |
|
|
|
to swim through the water. |
|
|
|
He'd reanimated the frog, brought its lower power back to |
|
|
|
life using electricity. |
|
|
|
And as a young female student who observed this, Wow. |
|
|
|
Because he would tour around with this electrical model showing |
|
|
|
people in different places. |
|
|
|
This young female student called Mary Shelley. |
|
|
|
This is amazing. |
|
|
|
I went off and wrote a rapid study, the story |
|
|
|
of Dr. Frankenstein, his monster drawing from those disgusting little |
|
|
|
frogs legs because you just extrapolated from the frog, say, |
|
|
|
well, if you could do this with an entire human |
|
|
|
body, disgusting. |
|
|
|
Why is this important for you? |
|
|
|
Today's is the first step forward to discovering that your |
|
|
|
brain works through electricity. |
|
|
|
The connections between these nerve cells, the brain and nerve |
|
|
|
fibres, pulse channel, electrical charge moves down those neurones, allowing |
|
|
|
things to happen, like allowing you to see, to hear |
|
|
|
all your sensation, allowing you to move. |
|
|
|
This comes from Kobani. |
|
|
|
A few years later, we move into Germany. |
|
|
|
We're going to move into Germany and France and go |
|
|
|
back and forward a bit. |
|
|
|
Yohannes Peter Muller is a German physiologist. |
|
|
|
He started to get obsessed with looking at different fish, |
|
|
|
different amphibians and discovering, Oh, they do these in a |
|
|
|
building on galvanise ideas. |
|
|
|
You could see how the nerve fibres are organised. |
|
|
|
And he started to systematise how we might organise in |
|
|
|
a more organised way. |
|
|
|
Our understanding of these brain structures went far beyond Vesalius, |
|
|
|
his books from 1500s. |
|
|
|
So now in the 1800s, around that time there's another |
|
|
|
man and he he would tool around as well. |
|
|
|
A little less charismatic, but still a great performer, Franz |
|
|
|
Francisco. |
|
|
|
And he had discovered he'd been interested in this idea |
|
|
|
that he met people who had different faculties. |
|
|
|
Some are really smart, some are really dumb, some he |
|
|
|
really quick. |
|
|
|
Some people were good at picking up language at different |
|
|
|
skills and noticed that their heads might be different shape. |
|
|
|
There was a lot of variation in Skull and he |
|
|
|
thought that maybe the way the brain maybe there's one |
|
|
|
way to to determine how the brain was related by |
|
|
|
measuring the skull from the outside, the bones in the |
|
|
|
way our brains are. |
|
|
|
So some of the big back of the head. |
|
|
|
So we were measuring lots of people's head in total |
|
|
|
detail of the bumps and what they were good at |
|
|
|
the first kind of assessments of IQ and, you know, |
|
|
|
memory function and language trying to measure these things as |
|
|
|
real move forward to science, but really wrong. |
|
|
|
But because nobody really believes that the skill at language |
|
|
|
isn't the best, just under AI or the obsequiousness, it's |
|
|
|
somewhere at the back of your head. |
|
|
|
Or empathy must be in this tiny bit. |
|
|
|
But we just know that it was just wrong. |
|
|
|
And he came to that through, you know, incorrectly correlating |
|
|
|
one set of variables, bumps and another performance in a |
|
|
|
very unsatisfactory way. |
|
|
|
This whole idea is term phrenology and it's still cropped |
|
|
|
up in the modern culture. |
|
|
|
Is Homer Simpson, with his brain obsessed with doughnuts and |
|
|
|
beer and sleep. |
|
|
|
You know, this is a pervasive pervaded to know this |
|
|
|
is another man's. |
|
|
|
We now go from Germany and France, goobers galore. |
|
|
|
A big fall is a big deal. |
|
|
|
You'll still find them. |
|
|
|
You go to Camden Market. |
|
|
|
Phrenology heads. |
|
|
|
You know, it's persisted till now, but this rather grumpy |
|
|
|
man, Jean-Pierre Florin, hated this idea. |
|
|
|
He really did not like goals proposal for the right |
|
|
|
reason that it was wrong. |
|
|
|
But he decided he would. |
|
|
|
He would really try and do experiments on it in |
|
|
|
stimulating these frogs is showing electricity. |
|
|
|
But what if we went into the brain and start |
|
|
|
to find out, do different bits of the brain do |
|
|
|
things differently? |
|
|
|
As ancient Egyptians had written about it, he didn't know. |
|
|
|
In 17 the early 1800s we found when he went |
|
|
|
in and started looking at damage to the brain, applying |
|
|
|
lesions was that it wasn't really some great kind of |
|
|
|
organisation. |
|
|
|
His lesions were pretty poorly done, they weren't well delivers. |
|
|
|
And he concluded The brain is just this mass. |
|
|
|
The upper part of a brain does all these amazing |
|
|
|
functions. |
|
|
|
It is not organised in a way though, that argues |
|
|
|
the language is in somewhere, etc. So he really in |
|
|
|
France was a big fight between these positions of organised |
|
|
|
versus non organised brains. |
|
|
|
So we've moved right down up to the top of |
|
|
|
this without coming to the latter part of the modern |
|
|
|
year period when things really changed with this man, Paul |
|
|
|
Broca, in France, same as Florence, around the same sort |
|
|
|
of time. |
|
|
|
He's an anatomist, a physician, anthropologist as well, and what |
|
|
|
he's famous for is discovering Brook, his area, funnily enough, |
|
|
|
named after him. |
|
|
|
And this is a picture of one of the disgusting, |
|
|
|
preserved brains that Brocket managed to take when he was |
|
|
|
doing his anatomy work. |
|
|
|
And what we can see in this brain is it's |
|
|
|
got a lot of damage. |
|
|
|
There was some event, some traumatic event. |
|
|
|
It caused damage to this particular area. |
|
|
|
And what Rocco realised was that he had a group |
|
|
|
of patients who had the same problem. |
|
|
|
They all couldn't speak properly. |
|
|
|
It could, they could understand language, but they just couldn't |
|
|
|
get the right words. |
|
|
|
Still very clever. |
|
|
|
He do lots of things, but they struggle with speech |
|
|
|
and he found all these patients with almost all cases |
|
|
|
had damaged. |
|
|
|
The right frontal lobes in this particular area might extend |
|
|
|
to other places. |
|
|
|
But the common area was this area known to termed |
|
|
|
Brock Azaria for the production of speech, and he established |
|
|
|
that in most humans the left hemisphere is critically important |
|
|
|
for language, which is still something that clinicians right now |
|
|
|
are somewhere in a hospital in London will be determining |
|
|
|
for somebody surgery. |
|
|
|
You do not want to do the wrong surgery and |
|
|
|
without someone's frontal lobe for language and discovered that they |
|
|
|
can't speak and surgically because it could be their right |
|
|
|
temporal lobe, one in a hundred is is actually doing |
|
|
|
that job. |
|
|
|
So it's a very critical thing. |
|
|
|
Still, the broker is important in your story because he |
|
|
|
is not the first to really say Florence is wrong, |
|
|
|
go. |
|
|
|
They're all wrong. |
|
|
|
But actually there is organisation in the brain. |
|
|
|
But we need to really think about the anatomy. |
|
|
|
We need to think about specifics. |
|
|
|
During this time in Germany, Karl Verner was also looking |
|
|
|
at patients and he discovered a bit of wrinkle. |
|
|
|
Verner Azaria I mean they named after him, which is |
|
|
|
more posterior to rock. |
|
|
|
Azaria Responsible for the comprehension of speech, and I put |
|
|
|
it in very simple terms, but generally these were the |
|
|
|
was the general idea. |
|
|
|
You've got language production and language comprehension in the left |
|
|
|
hemisphere in two particular areas. |
|
|
|
And both these men really led the way into the |
|
|
|
world of neuropsychology, of relating psychological function, speech reduction to |
|
|
|
speech comprehension, these two different abilities you might have. |
|
|
|
So. |
|
|
|
Yes, he was an incredible. |
|
|
|
These two men are incredible in advancing our understanding of |
|
|
|
brain and behaviour, roots change and development of neuropsychology. |
|
|
|
And this was going on in in France for for |
|
|
|
four. |
|
|
|
We go back to Brock having gone in in France |
|
|
|
for Paul Brock and in Germany for Calvin. |
|
|
|
And just you could walk there in 10 minutes down |
|
|
|
the road in Queen Square. |
|
|
|
John Healings Jackson is the other major figure in the |
|
|
|
understanding of brain and behaviour this time. |
|
|
|
So now a little bit for the mid 1800s, sort |
|
|
|
of late 1800s. |
|
|
|
And John healings Jackson. |
|
|
|
Key takeaways. |
|
|
|
You provide a whole range of important insights into the |
|
|
|
cerebral, the cerebral cortex and study of these brain damaged |
|
|
|
patients. |
|
|
|
A vast range that I could list, you know, colour, |
|
|
|
vision, sensation, spatial orientation, whole range of functions. |
|
|
|
And his boss, the sitting in the National Hospital for |
|
|
|
Neurology and Neurosurgery, where he worked for his career running |
|
|
|
a a ward when he was dedicated to not only |
|
|
|
helping patients, but making these discoveries about the brain, how |
|
|
|
it's organised. |
|
|
|
And so we have him to thank for a lot |
|
|
|
of key neuropsychological discoveries, by the way our brains are |
|
|
|
organised. |
|
|
|
Now one of the most critical cases we're going to |
|
|
|
come into a number of in this short lecture, we're |
|
|
|
coming to two really famous cases against the celebrities who |
|
|
|
made the key discoveries like Rocker and Veronica and John |
|
|
|
Hughes Jackson. |
|
|
|
But here's a man called Phineas Gauge. |
|
|
|
I'm sure many of you in the audience may have |
|
|
|
come across Phineas Gauge, but if you haven't, let me |
|
|
|
introduce this remarkable story. |
|
|
|
We don't really know that the urologist should have more |
|
|
|
credit for the person who discovered in his details. |
|
|
|
But it seems a move between different doctors and it's |
|
|
|
a bit disputed. |
|
|
|
The Phineas Gauge. |
|
|
|
Here is a picture of his skull and a picture |
|
|
|
of figure one in the paper of an iron bar |
|
|
|
used for railway sleeper movers. |
|
|
|
A Tiffany's gauge was a railroad worker. |
|
|
|
He was responsible for moving around, dealing with the the |
|
|
|
laying down a railway track. |
|
|
|
At that time, they had these huge metal poles that |
|
|
|
would allow you to move the big sleepers and move |
|
|
|
the big rails. |
|
|
|
You're putting a rope. |
|
|
|
You needed something really heavy this time. |
|
|
|
They're also using explosives. |
|
|
|
They're using because they had to cut through the rock. |
|
|
|
And for Phineas Gauge, a very unfortunate accident occurred or |
|
|
|
was working away. |
|
|
|
An explosion occurred on one of these steel rods just |
|
|
|
blown right up through his head and lodged inside his |
|
|
|
head. |
|
|
|
He should have died. |
|
|
|
He should have died. |
|
|
|
But the fact the reason he didn't was the speed |
|
|
|
of the of this rod penetrated his skull so fast, |
|
|
|
it sort of it didn't cause the surrounding damage in |
|
|
|
the way of a slow moving object. |
|
|
|
So remarkably, they were able to remove this pole. |
|
|
|
And he went on for a month or so for |
|
|
|
some period afterwards alive before he died. |
|
|
|
And what was remarkable in the case of Phineas Gauge |
|
|
|
was that he retains more or less his knowledge of |
|
|
|
who people were. |
|
|
|
He could see things all sensation movement, a lot of |
|
|
|
these general human properties, you know, things we take for |
|
|
|
granted. |
|
|
|
He could still do them despite this massive damage he'd |
|
|
|
had. |
|
|
|
What he lost was his ability to make careful decisions, |
|
|
|
to plan for the future, and his personality was really |
|
|
|
disrupted. |
|
|
|
So he was he would curse is violence. |
|
|
|
His entire sense of who he was, the people who |
|
|
|
knew him said he's not the same as change. |
|
|
|
So from that discovery of Phineas gauge, along with Brauchler |
|
|
|
and Guernica, we had this and John Ewing's Jackson, these |
|
|
|
discoveries about bits of the brain, it seems the frontal |
|
|
|
lobes. |
|
|
|
I'll be going through the anatomy on Friday show you |
|
|
|
where the frontal lobes are, unsurprisingly in the front of |
|
|
|
the brain. |
|
|
|
But these frontal lobes are critical for that kind of |
|
|
|
personality and thinking. |
|
|
|
And and sadly, people went on to run frontal lobotomies |
|
|
|
all around the world. |
|
|
|
Popping out of these for periods is a very unpleasant |
|
|
|
period in history that we may come back to later |
|
|
|
in the course. |
|
|
|
Paul Burgess in the last lecture will talk about the |
|
|
|
history of frontal lobotomies. |
|
|
|
So have Phineas Gauge railway worker and let's just listen |
|
|
|
to what I've said there. |
|
|
|
So he survived his personality disorders. |
|
|
|
So what I've been talking to now is Rocco, Veronica, |
|
|
|
John who these Jackson cases often escape each one of |
|
|
|
these people in cases giving us real insight into the |
|
|
|
organisation and function there has persisted. |
|
|
|
And to this day we still people look for this |
|
|
|
area. |
|
|
|
People still think this is true. |
|
|
|
What distinctly different Phineas gauge. |
|
|
|
Unlike the previous hundred years before, there was a bit |
|
|
|
of a mass, but it's around this time in the |
|
|
|
1800s that we have this shift towards more careful experimental |
|
|
|
science. |
|
|
|
So Jean-Pierre Florins, I'll go back and see him again. |
|
|
|
This is Jean-Pierre Florence that instigated in France, this idea. |
|
|
|
We need to experiment to understand the brain. |
|
|
|
We can't just observe in the period following these 33 |
|
|
|
men and this particular case, they're all about observing. |
|
|
|
But at this late 1800s, we had these three men |
|
|
|
that then revolutionised our understanding of brain function because they |
|
|
|
went back to the idea Galvani had produced and Mary |
|
|
|
Shelley had popularised in Frankenstein. |
|
|
|
What friction hits segue on the right. |
|
|
|
Everyone had big beards back then on the right in |
|
|
|
Germany. |
|
|
|
We were running a physiology laboratory but able to show |
|
|
|
it, start showing that they had much more improved. |
|
|
|
They didn't need a huge house to create batteries anymore. |
|
|
|
You could get small systems for electrically shocking and working |
|
|
|
with electricity. |
|
|
|
This is 1800s. |
|
|
|
So they were able to show that you could stimulate |
|
|
|
bits of the brain and cause movement to occur on |
|
|
|
the left here. |
|
|
|
David Farrier was a physician, is a Scottish physician who |
|
|
|
worked with John Healings. |
|
|
|
Jackson's became known as a student who. |
|
|
|
Again, 10 minutes away, you walked to where David Faraday |
|
|
|
would walk around doing things in the ward, helping patients. |
|
|
|
But he got a grant to start doing this experimental |
|
|
|
work by stimulating, and he was extremely systematic in that. |
|
|
|
And he he produced the first publication publication map of |
|
|
|
where what happens when you stimulate where in the brain. |
|
|
|
So if I stimulate above here in lots of different |
|
|
|
in various different animals, I'll get certain reactions, certain movements |
|
|
|
will occur. |
|
|
|
And they were really interested in this, and not just |
|
|
|
to make the fundamental discoveries we're talking about here. |
|
|
|
They wanted to understand epilepsy, and they could see that |
|
|
|
if you stimulate electricity in places you've got, movements are |
|
|
|
very much like an epileptic fits. |
|
|
|
So they're able to argue that epilepsy is this overreaction |
|
|
|
to much electrical charge through the brain from these early |
|
|
|
experiments. |
|
|
|
But it is a very sad tale because they were |
|
|
|
not using antiseptic techniques. |
|
|
|
These came in. |
|
|
|
So the experiments were really traumatic and indeed really drove |
|
|
|
in the United Kingdom, the Anti Vivisection Society. |
|
|
|
And to this day in the UK, we have some |
|
|
|
of the most stringent laws around the protection of animals |
|
|
|
do anywhere that animals. |
|
|
|
And there's lots of animal work going on at UCL. |
|
|
|
It's so carefully regulated and looked at with great care |
|
|
|
to make sure that every every effort is made to |
|
|
|
minimise any pain to any animals. |
|
|
|
But back then, unfortunately things were not so good. |
|
|
|
So reading these articles is somewhat is is not pleasant. |
|
|
|
Unfortunately, this is the history of historically. |
|
|
|
These two men, they move out of they move from |
|
|
|
the study of stimulating brains and damaging brains and so |
|
|
|
on to what happens if you look down a microscope. |
|
|
|
This is going on simultaneously. |
|
|
|
In the late 18th, we know in the early 1900s |
|
|
|
how on the right and Emilio Golgi, two men who |
|
|
|
had developed staining techniques. |
|
|
|
You can see how this picture I showed you earlier |
|
|
|
on the right and comedian Goldie's picture on the left. |
|
|
|
These are the drawings of anatomy. |
|
|
|
A key things that they had discovered that they could |
|
|
|
use, particular staining techniques that when you stay in a |
|
|
|
bit of brain wash it. |
|
|
|
I you can see it under microscope. |
|
|
|
Now Golgi and his name is all over cells. |
|
|
|
Views on any cell biology, any or any biology really, |
|
|
|
or probably where the Golgi apparatus inside a cell. |
|
|
|
This is this, you know, larger than life Italian. |
|
|
|
He put his name on it and he he discovered |
|
|
|
an enormous amount of ourselves by very carefully looking at |
|
|
|
microscopes. |
|
|
|
But what he thought when he looked at the brain |
|
|
|
was it was all the kind of big fibrous mass |
|
|
|
like a sponge. |
|
|
|
You know, sponges are one big kind of fibrous mass. |
|
|
|
And how awful this was wrong, how it looked in |
|
|
|
lots of sections. |
|
|
|
And he could see quite clearly from his notes that |
|
|
|
there appeared to be individual nerve cells that were not |
|
|
|
connected up and they were growing. |
|
|
|
And how argued this hypothesis that a brain is composed |
|
|
|
of neurones is something we take for granted. |
|
|
|
But it's only 200 years. |
|
|
|
All these amazing scientists before that were not aware of |
|
|
|
the existence of neurones. |
|
|
|
It's quite amazing to think of. |
|
|
|
They both received the Nobel Prize for this and they |
|
|
|
went up on the stage to collect it. |
|
|
|
It was all agreed in this period that pretty much |
|
|
|
all scientists agree with the Nobel that the position of |
|
|
|
neurones. |
|
|
|
But apparently Golgi did not like this idea and his |
|
|
|
entire Nobel speech to the community carried on arguing against |
|
|
|
the hypothesis which really pissed off. |
|
|
|
How does he come to collect the prize for the |
|
|
|
same thing? |
|
|
|
And they never really spoke to each other. |
|
|
|
He had a real falling out over this idea, but |
|
|
|
they made incredible advances and move forward this idea and |
|
|
|
we looking a lot in this course of neurones. |
|
|
|
So these are the guys behind the insights around this |
|
|
|
time just a bit earlier. |
|
|
|
In fact, we have Hermann von Helmholtz. |
|
|
|
We're looking at neurones and we're starting to you have |
|
|
|
rokkr looking at brains, but we have this incredible switch |
|
|
|
to empiricism. |
|
|
|
So if you do a psychology degree or neuroscience degree, |
|
|
|
you're taught to take a problem, you reduce it down, |
|
|
|
you systematically study it, you've hairy properties of the world |
|
|
|
and you statistically test them. |
|
|
|
And we have a lot of that in science. |
|
|
|
There is a vision, perception of space and so on. |
|
|
|
Where we're being examined by Helmholtz back then, the world |
|
|
|
was not so big. |
|
|
|
So these people neutral. |
|
|
|
Helmholtz was David Faraday had worked with Helmholtz. |
|
|
|
It was dialogue with John Giddings. |
|
|
|
Jackson How was aware of how they were all in |
|
|
|
cahoots in some ways, but disagreeing in agreement. |
|
|
|
So it was a very productive time in terms of |
|
|
|
changes. |
|
|
|
Now we come to the 1900s. |
|
|
|
We have colleagues shown there are neurones, but in Oxford |
|
|
|
we have Charles Scott Sherrington and his work were moving |
|
|
|
into the 1900. |
|
|
|
This is the 20th century. |
|
|
|
Science is now changing big time. |
|
|
|
The methods you can employ, the precision of instruments, what |
|
|
|
you can do. |
|
|
|
Psychology and neuroscience have become topics that are investigated more. |
|
|
|
And Charles Scott Sherrington In history, neuroscience is really important |
|
|
|
for rolling out the first fundamental explanation of how brains |
|
|
|
operate. |
|
|
|
Jake Deka had written about reflexes and movement, but just |
|
|
|
sketched it. |
|
|
|
You saw the diagrams. |
|
|
|
Sherrington had detailed diagrams and explanations. |
|
|
|
He worked out that there was something wrong. |
|
|
|
He went to how far stick the work around this |
|
|
|
time, how information must flow through the nerve system, how |
|
|
|
these with the stimulation of the frogs legs discovered there |
|
|
|
was a gap in the timing. |
|
|
|
Something was missing. |
|
|
|
These cells should foster. |
|
|
|
And he speculated that there must be some gap between |
|
|
|
the cells, which he called the sign ups that were |
|
|
|
some slowing and that stayed very controversial for 50 years |
|
|
|
or so. |
|
|
|
Fighting over the existence of sign ups did or did |
|
|
|
not operate throughout tricity or chemistry or books. |
|
|
|
Charles Scott Sharon's and was really the foundational figure in |
|
|
|
terms of laying out the general principles of the nervous |
|
|
|
system. |
|
|
|
We have to thank for that. |
|
|
|
We're going to wrap up now the last two people |
|
|
|
for the end of the lecture, and here we go |
|
|
|
again. |
|
|
|
I think charisma carries through a lot of these. |
|
|
|
We have not an Italian charismatic, but a Canadian. |
|
|
|
We've got a Canadian neuroscientist while the Penfield, while the |
|
|
|
penfield was drawing all that knowledge of fairy friction hits. |
|
|
|
It's shown and very mapped out where you should stimulate |
|
|
|
in animals to get certain behaviours. |
|
|
|
What Penfield argued was that let's use that in our |
|
|
|
surgery in Montreal, Montreal Institute back in this period of |
|
|
|
the 19, maybe 1930, through to the through to even |
|
|
|
now, incredible institute for insights into psychology and neuroscience, they |
|
|
|
started to do much more careful work to have a |
|
|
|
more experimental brain surgery where they would remove whole sets |
|
|
|
of bits of the brain to cure things like epilepsy |
|
|
|
or intractable pain, all sorts of things. |
|
|
|
What you want you want to do neurosurgery. |
|
|
|
What I said earlier with Broca's case, you do not |
|
|
|
want to disrupt language and someone can't speak. |
|
|
|
It's an utter devastation. |
|
|
|
If they can do lots, if they lose other things, |
|
|
|
not ever. |
|
|
|
You know, if I can't find my way, I can |
|
|
|
still get Google Maps or someone to tell. |
|
|
|
I can't speak to you. |
|
|
|
It's a disaster. |
|
|
|
So Penfield was very carefully using electrical stimulation in his |
|
|
|
surgeries to then make big discoveries about the brain. |
|
|
|
He would go in and stimulate and he discovered there's |
|
|
|
an entire map in the human brain of the of |
|
|
|
our body where sensations occurred. |
|
|
|
So what Penfield would do is have someone in surgery |
|
|
|
for the first time awake and careful surgical techniques, open |
|
|
|
up their skull, go into their brain while they're able |
|
|
|
to speak. |
|
|
|
There's no pain receptors in your brain. |
|
|
|
They're all on the outside. |
|
|
|
If you can anaesthetise all that, get it all set, |
|
|
|
you can go in and stimulate. |
|
|
|
And they still do this? |
|
|
|
No, not quite the way he did, but then much |
|
|
|
more carefully that he's able to stimulate and see what |
|
|
|
people could tell him. |
|
|
|
Things when you stimulate here. |
|
|
|
Oh, my hand starts itching, I stimulate there. |
|
|
|
Their knee will start to tingle. |
|
|
|
You can map all this out and discover that it |
|
|
|
was a very organised map of our human body or |
|
|
|
surfaces inside our brain running along this area of the |
|
|
|
Samantha sensory cortex. |
|
|
|
I'll talk about that on Friday in more detail. |
|
|
|
There was also a map of if you stimulate in |
|
|
|
other places, they might start moving their hand, twitching, a |
|
|
|
finger, moving their mouth, their lips. |
|
|
|
And you can see one of his drawings there of |
|
|
|
the these bits of the body. |
|
|
|
And it wasn't just organised 1 to 1 go to |
|
|
|
the tongue and the lips take up a huge number |
|
|
|
of cells in your brain. |
|
|
|
This will be true of all of you in the |
|
|
|
audience, your brain. |
|
|
|
Well, lots of brain cells. |
|
|
|
In fact, somebody once commented The number of brain cells |
|
|
|
in your as a human to move your finger is |
|
|
|
more than the whole brain cells in a mouse is |
|
|
|
so complicated human organising can move. |
|
|
|
We have a lot of support cells we have a |
|
|
|
lot of a lot of cells in the human brain. |
|
|
|
So there is some huge amounts of of cells in |
|
|
|
there organising our, our sensation in action, the last personal |
|
|
|
ones. |
|
|
|
And today's lecture as we come to the end is |
|
|
|
a woman. |
|
|
|
So she had a lot of bearded men so far, |
|
|
|
these people who had the power to do what they |
|
|
|
could do. |
|
|
|
The Montreal Neurological Institute in the 1940s, 30, 40 years |
|
|
|
going forward into the fifties is a change in women |
|
|
|
are alive in medicine in fact show Charles Scott showing |
|
|
|
some is one of the earliest people to really fight |
|
|
|
to allow women into medicine they were not allowed to |
|
|
|
study. |
|
|
|
You know, when these historical unpleasantries UCLA was actually pretty |
|
|
|
good at this changing things. |
|
|
|
But here we have Brenda milner, who's 104 years old |
|
|
|
at the moment, still alive, still has an office somewhere. |
|
|
|
Her office is covered with every possible accolade you could |
|
|
|
have in science. |
|
|
|
And below her is a normal brain. |
|
|
|
On the right, on the left is the brain of |
|
|
|
a patient called. |
|
|
|
Henry Lawson, who died about ten years ago, was throughout |
|
|
|
her career known as Patient H.M.. |
|
|
|
So Phineas Gauge is famous because that's what was going |
|
|
|
through his head. |
|
|
|
Hey, Jim was famous because a young maverick neurosurgeon removed |
|
|
|
his temporal lobe bilaterally. |
|
|
|
You can see in the diagram here, he removed all |
|
|
|
that brain tissue there and there, there, that huge amount |
|
|
|
of brain tissue all scrutinised very carefully. |
|
|
|
He survived perfectly healthy, lived for decades after the surgery, |
|
|
|
and the surgery worked for what he was trying to |
|
|
|
do. |
|
|
|
It stopped. |
|
|
|
H.M. had epilepsy, really severe epilepsy. |
|
|
|
It stopped. |
|
|
|
It was great, but it unfortunately did leave H.M utterly |
|
|
|
amnesic. |
|
|
|
After the surgery. |
|
|
|
He never learned a single thing about anyone he met. |
|
|
|
He couldn't remember any new details. |
|
|
|
He couldn't find his way around the hospital. |
|
|
|
He could remember what had happened to him long before, |
|
|
|
but any new memories were unable to be laid down. |
|
|
|
That bit of the capacity or other intellectual function, his |
|
|
|
knowing language, IQ planning, he could do all the things |
|
|
|
for this case couldn't. |
|
|
|
His personality stayed the same. |
|
|
|
But his memory had disappeared. |
|
|
|
And Brenda milner really was the pioneer who brought forward |
|
|
|
that and many other discoveries systematically about brain function. |
|
|
|
So we'll come back to Brenda milner when it comes |
|
|
|
on to our our next hour lectures on memory and |
|
|
|
learning systems. |
|
|
|
So today, we've gone through a whole sequence of celebrities |
|
|
|
of the brain all the way through our ancient past, |
|
|
|
Socrates. |
|
|
|
So, Brenda milner, on Friday, I'll start to dive into |
|
|
|
the neuroanatomy that underlies what we now know about the |
|
|
|
brain. |
|
|
|
To see you next Friday and enjoy the course. |
|
|
|
|